Skip to main content

Behavioural inventory of the giraffe (Giraffa camelopardalis)

Abstract

Background

Numerous factors like continuous habitat reduction or fragmentation for free-ranging giraffes (Giraffa camelopardalis) as well as e.g. suboptimal housing conditions for animals in captivity might lead to behavioural alterations as part of the overall adaptation process to the changing living conditions. In order to facilitate current and future studies on giraffe behaviour, a comprehensive ethogram was compiled based on existing literature, as well as observations on giraffes in the wild (Hwange National Park, Zimbabwe; Entabeni Game Reserve, South Africa), and in captivity (National Zoological Gardens of South Africa, Pretoria).

Findings

The resulting ethogram lists 65 different behavioural patterns, which were described and grouped into seven categories: General activities, Abnormal repetitive behaviours, General interactions, Bull-Cow behaviour, Bull-Bull behaviour, Cow-Bull behaviour, Maternal behaviours, and Interactions by calves. The behaviours were further described regarding a presumed purpose, particularly with respect to social interactions and sexual behaviour. Contradictory descriptions from previous studies were considered and discussed in comparison with our own observations.

Conclusions

This ethogram provides a basis for current and future studies by suggesting a terminology which can be used for harmonizing behavioural observations, thus helping to facilitate comparability of future results. Subsequently, a better understanding of the behavioural ecology of giraffes in the wild as well as in captivity could aid future conservation efforts.

Background

The giraffe (Giraffa camelopardalis) is the tallest land-living animal and the only extant species of its genus [1]. Although there is still uncertainty about the exact number and distribution of subspecies within Giraffa, a division into nine subspecies are generally accepted [2]. Two of these subspecies are currently listed as endangered by the International Union for Conservation of Nature and Natural Resources [3]. The giraffe naturally inhabits a variety of habitats, from deserts to more heavily vegetated bush- and tree savannah [4, 5], and there is evidence for habitat-related adjustments of occurring social structures and of particular behaviours, such as stable social structures and dominance hierarchies instead of fission-fusion structures [6].

Over the past couple of decades, the number of giraffes has declined considerably across Africa, presumably due to direct and indirect anthropogenic impact, such as extensive poaching, habitat destruction, and rinderpest [79]. As a consequence, several of today’s giraffe populations are isolated and live in detached habitat fragments or fenced reserves [7, 8]. By restricting the natural tendency of giraffes to roam vast areas in search for conspecifics, further implications in terms of behavioural alterations are easily conceivable [6, 10, 11]. These alterations might even have long-term effects on e.g. intraspecific competition, predator-prey relationships, or parasite transmission amongst other factors [6, 10, 11], and might have to be considered in future conservation plans for affected populations. However, data on the giraffe’s ethology and its variation should be available in order to provide information for an integrated conservation approach [12].

Monitoring of wildlife behaviour is also a valuable and frequently used tool to provide information about the health and welfare status of animals in captivity [13, 14]. However, the interpretation of behavioural data is not robust, and relies heavily on preliminary defined criteria [15]. In order to obtain reliable results, the respective behavioural patterns must be adequately defined [16]. To allow cross-institutional comparison of collected data and to contribute to a reliable base of information, behaviour must be measured in a distinct and standardised way. Thus, the use of an accurate established ethogram is highly recommendable, not least because it helps to prevent ‘drift’ during the course of observation and also in order to facilitate methodology and results [17].

In terms of available behavioural data for the giraffe, many of the contributing studies only cover specific behavioural classes and at times, these studies use inconsistent terminology or innovate purpose-built definitions for certain behaviours e.g. [1823]. Hence, data to build upon is rather limited. In this paper, we therefore aim to provide a full descriptive catalogue of the giraffe’s behavioural repertoire for observations of wild and captive animals. The compiled ethogram is structured into several categories, which are, in the case of intraspecific interactions, subdivided by sex and roughly predefined age classes of the acting individuals, as well as the animals the behaviour is presumably directed to. The definitions and general remarks provided will hopefully be of practical value in terms of producing more comparable ethological data in the future.

Observations were conducted at three different study sites: Hwange National Park (HNP), Zimbabwe, Entabeni Game Reserve (EGR), South Africa, and at the National Zoological Gardens of South Africa (NZG) in Pretoria.

Findings

Methods

General method

In a similar approach to other studies e.g. [24, 25], the behaviours reported in this paper were compiled from several sources. In order to assess as many of the behaviours shown by giraffes as possible, numerous peer-reviewed articles, dissertations and theses, and other publications (Appendix), focussing on descriptions of giraffe behaviour or at least partly addressing the topic, were reviewed for behavioural descriptions and definitions e.g. [2, 4, 5, 9, 16, 1823, 2650]. In addition, observations were conducted in three different environments, in order to confirm, refine, and if necessary extend existing descriptions of giraffe behaviour. In case of future observations though, variations in methodology and flexibility of the research has to be noted.

All behaviours were described as brief and definite as possible, according to the descriptions by other authors and our own observations. In this regard, we tried to comment regarding the behaviours apparent social and/or ecological context, and assumed purpose. The resulting list of behaviours is divided into two main groups; Activities (characterised by the absence of any social context) and Interactions (characterised by the presence of some kind of interaction between animals).

Literature review

In total, 104 publications (93 scientific articles, 2 books, 3 PhD theses, 2 MSc dissertations and 4 other publications) on giraffe behaviour, ecology, and general biology were reviewed for descriptions of behavioural patterns in wild and captive giraffes, listed in the Appendix.

Original observations

Giraffes were predominantly observed using ad-libitum and all-occurrence sampling [51]. As an example of a near-natural environment, wild giraffes were observed in HNP for thirteen weeks, between November and December 2010, and from March to April in 2011. During a total observation time of 272 hours, 1264 sightings were recorded (345 sightings of mature males, 752 of mature females, 159 of juveniles / subadult individuals). HNP covers 14.650 km2 in western Zimbabwe and is entirely unfenced. A presumably stable population of roughly 2800 giraffes are estimated to live in HNP and adjacent areas [3]. Lions as the giraffe’s main predators are abundant [52], and also other large predators such as spotted hyenas, cheetahs and leopards are present. Observations were conducted at several water holes and open plains in the Main Camp area, ranging from Guvalala Pan to Ngweshla Pan.

About 40 giraffes were additionally observed on a daily basis in EGR for three weeks in September 2011. EGR is a private game reserve, covering 250 km2 and is entirely fenced. An otherwise unmanaged population of about 45 giraffes were kept in the reserve during the time of observation. Lions, cheetahs, and leopards are also present.

In order to collect data on an abnormal repetitive behaviour in a captive animal, two adult giraffes (1 male, 1 female) housed at NZG, were also briefly observed for 7 hours in February 2011.

Results and Discussion

A total of 65 different behaviours could be identified. These behaviours were subdivided into 30 Activities and 35 Interactions. Activities were subdivided further into General activities (Additional file 1: Table S1) and Abnormal repetitive behaviours (Additional file 2: Table S2). Interactions were structured by sex and age class of the acting animal, and of the animals the behaviour is presumably directed to. This resulted in General interactions (Additional file 3: Table S3), Bull-Cow behaviour (Additional file 4: Table S4), Bull-Bull behaviour (Additional file 5: Table S5), Cow-Bull behaviour (Additional file 6: Table S6), interactions by calves (Additional file 7: Table S7), and maternal behaviours (Additional file 8: Table S8). No behaviours were found being performed exclusively between cows.

Activities

As mentioned above, behaviours allocated to the category Activities are not related to any type of interactive behaviour and also not restricted to one sex or age class. Behaviours of the Activities category were further subdivided into General Activities (Additional file 1: Table S1) and Abnormal repetitive behaviours (Additional file 2: Table S2).

Abnormal repetitive behaviours

As in other species, it is assumed that abnormal repetitive behaviours often develop in captive animals due to a time budget shift in the daily activity pattern [46, 47]. Giraffes in captivity spend considerably less time feeding compared to the amount of time giraffes browse in the wild [16, 20].

Interactions

This section includes behaviours which are characterised by any type of direct or indirect social interaction between individual giraffes. Behaviours of the Interactions category were further subdivided into General Interactions (Additional file 3: Table 3), Bull - Cow Behaviour (Additional file 4: Table S4), Bull - Bull Behaviour (Additional file 5: Table S5), Cow - Bull Behaviour (Additional file 6: Table S6), behavioural Interactions by Calves (Additional file 7: Table S7) and maternal behaviours (Additional file 8: Table S8). All behaviours performed between cows (cow-cow) were also observed between other constellations of sex and age, thus listed under general interactions.

This ethogram was compiled to serve as a basis for current and future studies designed to further examine the complex behavioural patterns of the species. Based on our own observations, several often older descriptions could be verified and even new insights added to what is stated in literature.

The classification of the described behaviours into activities and interactions might appear rather clear from a definition point of view, but should be used with precaution, because the complete intention and purpose of an observed behaviour always remains an interpretation based on a projection of the observer’s conception. The animal’s behaviour can not be reduced to the sum of different behavioural acts, which is why clear and precise terminology is essential to create a common language understandable among human observers and to contribute to the understanding of wildlife behaviour.

Regarding social interactions not restricted to one sex or age class (General Interactions), it is worth noting that many of these behaviours were originally described as exclusively exaggerated by one sex, or by a specific age class. However, during our observations, we also register the performance of these behaviours by the respective opposite sex, or across age classes, respectively.

The section on play behaviour was kept rather short and comprehensive. For the sake of brevity, all behaviours of the same obvious (play) intention were summarised. Nevertheless, future studies might be able to reveal various forms of play behaviour in giraffe, similar to that of other ungulates, although probably not as pronounced as e.g. in horses [24].

Several behaviours, although often only observed in form of an attempt (e.g. mounting, mating, nursing) are classified as separate behaviours in this ethogram, because attempts seem to be distinct and important, therefore these behaviours might be considered as a separate sub-section in an ethogram used for observations.

It must be also mentioned, that for the visual communication of dominance, contradictory descriptions are given in literature. Pratt and Anderson [5] report that a dominant bull will walk towards an opponent with its head held high, intending to look as big as possible. On the contrary, Dagg [9] states that a dominant bull, threatening an opponent will carry his head deep with the neck parallel to the ground, as if assuming a fighting position. We suggest that both observations are adequate and that communication of dominance might vary with the distance between opponents. In this regard, the “head-high” posture could be assumed for a distance of more then two body lengths, while the “fight” posture would be assumed with the opponent in close proximity, as it has been seen during our own observations. The typical intention of a threatening giraffe bull is often expressed by an arched and tensed neck (see Dominance gesture), as it is also seen in other ungulates, e.g. horses [53] or reindeer [54]. The visual communication of submission is contrary to that of dominance and thus is also described contradictory in literature. According to Pratt and Anderson [5, 27], the subdominant individual will carry its head low to look smaller than it is, in order to not provoke aggression. Dagg [9] reports that inferior giraffe bulls stand with an erect neck and the nose pointed upwards, assuming a feeding position and thereby exposing the body to attacks. As well as for dominance, a distance dependent expression for submission might be considered. In this regard, the plasticity of social behaviour and communication patterns should be borne in mind during conduction and interpretation of behavioural observations.

Conclusions

Observed behaviours should be interpreted carefully and the researcher should consider the animal’s intention not only for the moment and place of observation but also on a larger temporal and spatial scale. An animal’s original intention is in many cases difficult to evaluate and rather oblique, which applies particularly to large animals like the giraffe with its rarely assessable visual facilities [26]. Furthermore, olfactory cues and insufficient recognition regarding long distance communication via infrasound make it sometimes difficult to unequivocally relate a certain observed behavioural event to a specific category (von Muggenthaler, Baes, Hill, Fulk, Lee, unpublished results), therefore the division of interactions and activities not related to a social context remain somewhat arbitrary.

A comprehensive and reliable tool to monitor giraffe behaviour in the wild as well as in captivity is a necessity to gain a better understanding of the giraffe’s life-history requirements. Subsequently, gaining a better understanding of giraffe behaviour will help to develop more effective conservation strategies for improving giraffe management in the wild and in captivity by creating species-tailored management plans.

Appendix

Sources used for the compilation of the giraffe ethogram

Awange JL, Aseto O, Ong’ang’a O (2004): A case study on the impact of giraffes in Ruma National Park, Kenya. Journal of Wildlife Rehabilitation 27, 16-21.

Backhaus D (1961): Beobachtungen an Giraffen in Zoologischen Gärten und freier Wildbahn. Instit. Pares. Nat. Cong. Ruanda-Urund, Bruxelles. 202 pp.

Bashaw MJ, Bloomsmith MA, Maple TL, Bercovitch FB (2007): The structure of social relationships among captive female giraffe (Giraffa camelopardalis). Journal of Comparative Psychology 121 No.1, 46-53.

Bashaw M (2010): Consistency of captive giraffe behaviour under two different management regimes. Zoo Biology 29, 1-8.

Baxter E & Plowman AB (2001): The effect of increasing fibre on feeding, rumination and oral stereotypies in captive giraffes (Giraffa camelopardalis). Animal Welfare 10, 281-290.

Bercovitch FB, Bashaw MJ, Penny CG, Rieches RG (2004): Maternal investment in captive giraffes. Journal of Mammalogy 85 No. 3, 428-431.

Bercovitch FB, Bashaw MJ, del Castillo SM (2006): Sociosexual behaviour, male mating tactics, and the reproductive cycle of giraffe Giraffa camelopardalis. Hormones and Behavior 50, 314-321.

Bercovitch FB & Berry PS (2009): Ecological determinants of her size in the Thornicroft’s giraffe in Zambia. African Journal of Ecology 48, 962-971.

Bercovitch FB & Berry PS (2009): Reproductive life history of Thornicroft‘s giraffe in Zambia. African Journal of Ecology 48, 535-538.

Bernhard A, Eulenberger K (2003): Hand-rearing of a giraffe (Giraffa camelopüardalis) at Leipzig zoo. Erkrankungen der Zootiere 41, 327-328.

Berry PS (1978): Range movements of giraffe in the Luangwa Valley, Zambia. East African Wildlife Journal 16, 77-83.

Bredin IP, Skinner JD, Mitchell G (2008): Can osteophagy provide giraffes with phosphorus and calcium? Onderstepoort Journal of Veterinary Research 75, 1-9.

Brenneman RA, Louis EEJr, Fennessy J (2009): Genetic structure of two populations of the Namibian Giraffe, Giraffa camelopardalis angolensis. African Journal of Ecology 47, 720-728.

Brenneman RA, Bagine, RK, Brown, DM, Ndetei R, Louis EEJr (2009): Implications of closed ecosystem conservation management: the decline of Rothschild’s giraffe (Giraffa camelopardalis rothschildi) in Lake Nakuru National Park, Kenya. African Journal of Ecology 47, 711-719.

Blomqvist PA & Renberg L (2007): Feeding behaviour of Giraffe (Giraffa camelopardalis) in Mokolodi Reserve, Botswana. University of Uppsala, Minor Fied Study.

Bourliere F (1961): The sex ratio of the giraffe. Mammalia 25, 467-471.

Brown DM, Brenneman RA, Koepfli KP, Pollinger JP, Milá B, Georgiadis NJ, Louis EEJr, Grether GF, Jacobs DK, Wayne RK (2007): Extensive population structure in the giraffe. BMC Biology. doi:10.1186/1741-7007-5-57.

Caister LE, Shields WM, Gosser A (2003): Female tannin avoidance: a possible explanation for habitat and dietary segregation of giraffes (Giraffa camelopardalis peralta) in Niger. African Jorunal of Ecology 41, 201-210.

Cameron EZ & du Toit J (2007): Winning by a neck: tall giraffes avoid competing with shorter browsers. American Naturalist 169, 130-135.

Cameron EZ & du Toit J. (2005): Social influence on vigilance behaviour in giraffes, Giraffa camelopardalis. Animal Behaviour 69, 1337-1344.

Coe MJ (1967): Necking behaviour in the Giraffe. Journal of Zoology, London 151, 313-321.

Ciofolo I, Ambouta K, Le Pendu Y (2009): Les dernières girafes d’Afrique de l’ouest: sauvegarde assure ou avenbir menacé? Rev. Écol. (Terre Vie) 64, 351-358.

Ciofolo I & Le Pendu Y (2002): The feeding behaviour of giraffe in Niger. Mammalia 66, 183-194.

Ciofolo I (1995): West Africa’s last giraffes: the conflict between development and conservation. Journal of Tropical Ecology 11, 577-588.

Claus M, Franz-Odendaal TA, Brasch J, Castell JC, Kaiser T (2007): Tooth wear in captive giraffes (Giraffa camelopardalis): Mesowear analysis classifies free-ranging specimens as browsers but captive ones as grazers. Journal of Zoo and Wildlife Medicine 38, 433-445.

Clauss M, Flach EJ, Lechner-Doll M, Hatt JM (2003) Reaction of a group of captive giraffe (Giraffa camelopardalis) to the introduction of a tannin-containing pellet. Erkrankungen der Zootiere 41, 343.

Dagg, AI (1962) The distribution of the giraffe in Africa. Mammalia 26, 497-505.

Dagg AI (1970): Tactile encounters in a Herd of Captive Giraffes. Journal of Mammalogy 51 No. 2, 279-287.

Dagg AI (1971): Giraffa camelopardalis. Mammalian species 5, 1-8.

Dagg AI & Taub A (1970): Flehmen. Mammalia 34 No. 4, 686-695.

del Castillo SM, Bashaw MJ, Patton ML, Rieches RR, Bercovitch FB (2005): Fecal steroid analysis of female giraffe (Giraffa camelopardalis) reproductive condition and the impact of endocrine status on daily time budgets. General and Comparative Endocrinology 141, 271-281.

Dumonceaus GA, Baumann JE, Camilo GR (2006): Evaluation of progesterone in feces of captive reticulated giraffe (Giraffa camelopardalis reticulate). Journal of Zoo and Wildlife Medicine 3,255-261.

Du Toit JT & Yetman CA (2005): Effects of body size on the diurnal activity budget of African browsing ruminants. Oecologia 143 No. 2, 317-325.

Fennessy J (2004): Ecology of desert-dwelling giraffe Giraffa camelopardalis angolensis in northwestern Namibia. University of Sydney, Australia, Phd thesis.

Fennessy J (2009): Home range and seasonal movements of Giraffa camelopardalis angolensis in the Northern Namib Desert. African Journal of Ecology 47 No. 3, 318-327.

Fennessy J & Brown D (2010): Giraffa camelopardalis. IUCN 2010. IUCN Red List of Threatened Species. Version 2010.3.

Fleming PA, Hofmeyr SD, Nicolson SW, du Toit JT (2006): Are giraffes pollinators of flower predators of Acacia nigrescens in Kruger National Park, South Africa? Journal of Tropical Ecology 22, 247-253.

Foster J & Dagg I (1972): Notes on the biology of the giraffe. East African Wildlife Journal 10, 1-16.

Foster JB (1966) The giraffe of Nairobi National Park: home ranges, sex ratios, the herd and food. East African Wildlife Journal 4, 139-148

Gilbert DE, Loskutoff NM, Dorn CG, Nemec LA, Calle PP, Kraemer DC, Threlfall WR, Raphael BL (1988): Hormonal manipulation and ultrsonographic monitoring of ovarian activity in the giraffe. Theriogenology 29, 248.

Ginnett TF, Demment MW (1997) Sex differences in giraffe foraging behaviour at two spatial scales. Oecologia 110, 291-300.

Ginnett TF, Demment MW (1999): Sexual segregation by Masai giraffes at two spatial scales. African Journal of Ecology 37, 93-106.

Giraffe Conservation Foundation: The Facts. URL: http://www.giraffeconservation.org/giraffe_facts.php?pgid=40 (accessed January 20, 2011).

Gombe S, Kayanja FI (1974): Ovarian progestins in Masai giraffe (Giraffa camelopardalis). Journal of Reproduction and Fertility 40, 45-50.

Goodman PS, Tomkinson AJ (1987): The past distribution of giraffe in Zululand and its implications for reserve management. South African Journal of Wildlife Research 17, 28-23.

Grubb P (2005): Artiodactyla. In: D. E. Wilson and D. M. Reeder (eds), Mammal Species of the World. A Taxonomic and Geographic Reference (3rd ed), 637-722. Johns Hopkins University Press, Baltimore, USA.

Hall-Martin AJ (1974): Notes on utilization of different vegetation types by giraffe. South African Journal of Science 70, No. 4, 122-123.

Hall-Martin AJ (1975): Studies on the biology and productivity of the giraffe, Giraffa camelopardalis. D.Sc. thesis, University of Pretoria.

Hall-Martin AJ, Skinner JD, van Dyk JM (1975): Reproduction in the giraffe in relation to some environmental factors. East African Wildlife Journal 13, 237-248.

Hall-Martin AJ, Skinner JD, Hopkins BJ (1978): The development of the reproductive organs of the male giraffe, Giraffa camelopardalis. Journal of Reproduction and Fertility 52, 1-7.

Hall-Martin AJ & Skinner JD (1978): Observations on puberty and pregnancy in female giraffe (Giraffa camelopardalis). South African Journal on Wildlife Research 8, 91-94.

Hassanin A, Ropiquet A, Gourmand AL, Chardonnet B, Rigoulet J (2007): Mitochondrial DNA variablity in Giraffa camelopardalis: consequences for taxonmoy, phlyogeography and conservation of giraffes in West and central Africa. C.R. Biologies 330, 265-274.

Horwich R, Ktichen C, Wangel M, Ruthe R (1983): Behavioral development in Okapis and Giraffes. Zoo Biology 2, 105-125.

Innis AC (1958): The behaviour of giraffe, Giraffa camelopardalis, in the eastern Transvaal. Proceedings of the Zoological Society, London 131, 245-275.

Isobe N, Nakao T, Shimada M, Fukumoto Y, Watanabe H, Minami S, Noda A, Yoshimura Y (2007) Fecal progestagen and estrone during pregnancy in a giraffe: a case report. Journal of Reproduction and Development 53, 159-164.

Jolly L (2003): Giraffe husbandry manual. URL: http://www.aszk.org.au/docs/giraffe.pdf (accessed September 29 2010).

Kok, OB, Opperman, DP (1980): Feeding behaviour of giraffe Giraffa camelopardalis in the Willem-Pretorius-Game-Reserve, Orange Free State. South African Journal of Wildlife Research 10, 45-55.

Kristal MB, Noonan M (1979): Note on sleep in captive giraffes (Giraffa camelopardalis reticulata). South African Journal of Zoology 14, 108.

Kristal MB, Noonan M (1979): Perinatal maternal and neonatal behaviour in the captive reticulated giraffe. South African Journal of Zoology 14, 103-107.

Kruger JW (1994): The feeding ecology and behaviour of re-introduced giraffe (Giraffa camelopardalis) in the Kalahari Gemsbok National Park. Msc Thesis, University of Pretoria.

Lamprey HF (1963): Ecological separation of the large mamal species in the Tarangire game reserve, Tanganyika. African Journal of Ecology 1, 63-92.

Langman VA (1978): Giraffe pica behaviour and pathology as indicator of nutritional stress. The Journal of Wildlife Management 42, 141-147.

Leuthold BM & Leuthold W (1972) Food habbits of giraffe in Tsavo National Park, Kenya. East African Wildlife Journal 10, 129-141.

Leuthold BM & Leuthold W (1978) Daytime activity patterns of gerenuk and giraffe in Tsavo National Park, Kenya. East African Wildlife Journal 16, 231-243.

Leuthold B (1979): Social organization and behaviour of giraffe in Tsavo East National Park. African Journal of Ecology 17, 19-34.

Leuthold BM & Leuthold W (1978): Ecology of giraffes in Tsavo-East National Park, Kenya. East African Wildlife Journal 16 No. 1, 1-20.

Le Pendu Y, Ciofolo I, Gosser A (2000): The social organization of giraffes in Niger. African Journal of Ecology 38,78-85.

Le Pendu Y & Ciofolo I (1999): Seasonal movements of giraffes in Niger. Journal of Tropical Ecology 15, 341-353.

Leroy R, de Visscher Ma, Halidou O, Boureima A (2009): The las African white giraffes live in farmer’s fields. Biodiversity and Conservation 18, 2663-2677.

Loskutoff NM, Walker L, Ott-Joslin JE, Raphael BL, Lasley BL (1986): Urinary steroid evaluations to monitor ovarian function in exotic ungulates: II. Comparison between the giraffe (Giraffa camelopardalis) and the Okapi (Okapia johnstoni). Zoo Biology 5, 331-338.

Lueders I, Niemuller C, Pootoolal J, Rich P, Gray C, Streich WJ, Hildebrandt TB (2009): Sonomorphology of the reproductive tract in male and pregnant and nonpregnant female Rothschild‘s giraffes (Giraffa camelopardalis rothschildi). Theriogenology 72, 22-31.

Lueders I, Hildebrandt TB, Pootoolal J, Rich P, Gray C (2009): Ovarian ultrasonography correlated with fecal progestins and estradiol during the estrous cycle and early pregnancy in giraffes (Giraffa camelopardalis rothschildi). Biology of Reproduction 81, 989-995.

Lydekker R (1904): On the subspecies of Giraffa camelopardalis. Proceedings of the Zoological Society of London 1, 202-207.

Martinez del Castillo G (2006): Correction of the hoofs due to abnormal growth in juvenile captive giraffes. Redvet 7, 2-6.

Mitchell G & Skinner JD (2003): On the origin, evolution and phylogeny of giraffes Giraffa camelopardalis. Transactions of the Society of South Africa 58 No. 1, 51-73.

Mitchell G, van Sittert SJ, Skinner JD (2009): Sexual selection is not the origin of long necks in giraffes. Journal of Zoology 278, 281-286.

Nowak RM (1999): Walker's Mammals of the World. Johns Hopkins University Press.

Nesbit Evans EM (1970) The reaction of a group of Rothschild’s giraffe to a new environment. East African Wildlife Journal 8, 53-62.

Parker DM (2004): The feeding biology and potential impact of introduced giraffe (Giraffa camelopardalis) in the Eastern Cape Province, South Africa. MSc thesis, Rhodes University.

Patten RA (1940): Breeding the giraffe (Giraffa camelopardalis). Australian Zoologist 9, 452-454.

Pellew RA (1983): The giraffe and its food resource in the Serengeti. I: Composition, biomass and production of available browse. African Journal of Ecology 21, 241-267.

Pellew RA (1983): The giraffe and its food resource in the Serengeti. II. Response of the giraffe population to changes in the food supply. African Journal of Ecology 21, 269-283.

Pellew RA (1984): Food consumption and energy budgets of the giraffe. Journal of Applied Ecology 21, No. 1, 141-159.

Périquet S, Valeix M, Loveridge AJ, Madzikanda H, MacDonald DW & Fritz H (2010): Individual vigilance of African herbivores while drinking: the role of immediate predation risk and context. Animal Behaviour 79, 665-671.

Phillips K (2006): How giraffes keep the pressure up. Journal of experimental Biology 209, iii.

Pournele GH (1955): Notes on the reproduction of a Baringo giraffe. Journal of Mammalogy 4, 574.

Pratt DM & Anderson VH (1982): Population, distribution and behaviour of giraffe in the Arusha National Park, Tanzania. Journal of Natural History 16, 481-489.

Pratt DM & Anderson VH (1985): Giraffe social behaviour. Journal of Natural History 19, 771-781.

Pratt DM & Anderson VH (1979): Giraffe cow-calf relationships and social development of the calf in the Serengeti. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie 51, 233-251.

Sauer JJ, Theron GK, Skinner JD (1977): Food preferences of giraffe Giraffa camelopardalis in the arid bushveld of the eastern Transvaal. South African Journal of Wildlife Research 7, 53-59.

Shorrocks B & Croft DP (2009): Necks and networks: a preliminary study of population structure in the reticulated giraffe (Giraffa camelopardalis reticulate de Winston). African Journal of Ecology 47, 374-381.

Seeber PA, Ndlovu HT, Duncan P, Ganswindt A (2012a): Grazing behaviour of the giraffe (Giraffa camelopardalis) in Hwange National Park, Zimbabwe. African Journal of Ecology, DOI: 10.1111/j.1365-2028.2011.01314.x

Suraud JP (2011): Identifying conservation constraints for the last West African giraffe: population dynamics determining factors and spatial distribution pattern. PhD thesis, University of Lyon.

Tarou Fernendez L, Bashaw MJ, Sartor RL, Bouwens NR, Maki TS (2008): Tongue twisters: feeding enrichment to reduce oral stereotypy in giraffe. Zoo Biology 27, 200-212.

Tarou L, Bashaw MJ, Maple TL (2000): Social attachment in giraffe: Response to social separation. Zoo Biology 19, 41-51.

van der Jeugd HP & Prins HT (2000): Movements and group structure of giraffe (Giraffa camelopardalis) in Lake Manyara National Park, Tanzania. Journal of Zoology 251, 15-21.

Veasey JS, Waran NK, Young RJ (1996): On comparing the behaviour of zoo housed animals with wild conspecifics as a welfare indicator, using the giraffe (Giraffa camelopardalis) as a model. Animal Welfare 5, 139-153.

von Muggenthaler E, Baes C, Fulk R, Lee A (1999): Infrasound and low frequency vocalizations from the giraffe; Helmholtz resonance in biology. Proceedings of Riverbanks Consortium.

Wang T, Brondum E, Hasenkam M, Secher N, Bertelsen M, Grondahl C, Kastberg K, Buhl R, Aalkjaer C, Baandrup U, Nygaard H, Smerup M, Sloth E, Nissen P, Runge M (2008): Blood flows and pressures when the giraffe lowers its head. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology 3, 107-108.

Western D (1971): Giraffe chewing a Grant’s gazelle carcass. East African Wildlife Journal 9, 156-157.

Woods TD (1972): The precopulatory behaviour in male giraffe. Lammergeyer 17, 67.

Wyatt JR (1971): Osteophagia in Masai giraffe. East African Wildlife Journal 9, 157.

Young TP & Isbell LA (1991): Sex differences in Giraffe feeding ecology: energetic and social constraints. Ethology 87, 79-89.

Yuan J, Dong G, Zhang D (2004): The activity pattern of giraffe (Giraffa camelopardalis) during winter. Chinese Journal of Zoology 39, 76-78.

References

  1. Mitchell G, Skinner JD: On the origin, evolution and phylogeny of giraffes Giraffa camelopardalis. Trans R Soc S Afr. 2003, 58: 51-73. 10.1080/00359190309519935.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Dagg AI, Foster JB: The Giraffe: Its Biology, Behaviour and Ecology. 1976, New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold

    Google Scholar 

  3. Fennessy J, Brown D: Giraffa Camelopardalis. IUCN 2011. IUCN Red List of Threatened Species. Version. 2011

    Google Scholar 

  4. Fennessy J: Home range and seasonal movement of Giraffa camelopardalis angolensis in the northern Namib desert. Afr J Ecol. 2009, 47: 318-327. 10.1111/j.1365-2028.2008.00963.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Pratt DM, Anderson VH: Population, distribution, and behaviour of giraffe in the Arusha National Park. Tanzania. J Nat Hist. 1982, 16: 481-489. 10.1080/00222938200770381.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. van der Jeugd H, Prins HH: Movements and group structure of giraffe (Giraffa camelopardalis) in Lake Manyara National Park. Tanzania. J Zool. 2000, 251: 15-21.

    Google Scholar 

  7. Ciofolo I: West Africa’s last giraffes: the conflict between development and conservation. J Trop Ecol. 1995, 11: 577-588. 10.1017/S0266467400009159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Dagg AI: The distribution of the giraffe in Africa. Mammalia. 1962, 26: 497-505.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Dagg AI: Giraffa camelopardalis. Mamm Species. 1971, 5: 1-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Le Pendu Y, Ciofolo I: Seasonal movements of giraffes in Niger. J Trop Ecol. 1999, 15: 341-353. 10.1017/S0266467499000863.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Brenneman RA, Bagine RK, Brown DM, Ndetei R, Louis EE: Implications of closed ecosystem conservation management: the decline of Rothschild’s giraffe (Giraffa camelopardalis rothschildi) in Lake Nakuru National Park. Kenya. Afr J Ecol. 2009, 47: 711-719. 10.1111/j.1365-2028.2008.01029.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Curio E: Conservation needs ethology. Trends Ecol Evol. 1996, 11: 260-263. 10.1016/0169-5347(96)20046-1.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  13. Maple TL: Toward a science of welfare for animals in the zoo. J Appl Anim Welfare Sci. 2007, 10: 63-70. 10.1080/10888700701277659.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  14. Clubb R, Mason G: Captivity effects on wide-ranging carnivores. Nature. 2003, 245: 473-474.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Castellote M, Fossa F: Measuring acoustic activity as a method to evaluate welfare in captive Beluga whales (Delphinapterus leucas). Aquat Mamm. 2006, 32: 325-333. 10.1578/AM.32.3.2006.325.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Veasey JS, Waran NK, Young RJ: On comparing the behaviour of zoo housed animals with wild conspecifics as a welfare indicator, using th giraffe (Giraffa camelopardalis) as a model. Anim Welfare. 1996, 5: 139-153.

    Google Scholar 

  17. Martin P, Bateson P: Measuring behaviour. An introductory guide. 2000, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press

    Google Scholar 

  18. Bashaw MJ: Consistency of captive giraffe behavior under two different management regimes. Zoo Biol. 2010, 29: 1-8.

    Google Scholar 

  19. Bercovitch F, Bashaw M, del Castillo S: Sociosexual behavior, male mating tactics, and the reproductive cycle of giraffe Giraffa camelopardalis. Horm Behav. 2006, 50: 314-321. 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2006.04.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Baxter E, Plowman AB: The effect of increasing dietary fibre on feeding, rumination and oral stereotypies in captive giraffe (Giraffa camelopardalis). Anim Welf. 2001, 10: 281-290.

    Google Scholar 

  21. Leuthold BM, Leuthold W: Daytime activity patterns of gerenuk and giraffe in Tsavo National Park. Kenya. East Afr Wildl J. 1978, 16: 231-243. 10.1111/j.1365-2028.1978.tb00444.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Nesbit Evans EM: The reaction of a group of Rothschild’s giraffe to a new environment. East Afr Wildl J. 1970, 8: 53-62. 10.1111/j.1365-2028.1970.tb00830.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Innis AC: The behaviour of the giraffe, Giraffa camelopardalis, in the Eastern Transvaal. Proc Zool Soc Lond. 1958, 131: 245-275.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. McDonnell SM, Poulin A: Equid play ethogram. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2002, 78: 263-290. 10.1016/S0168-1591(02)00112-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Lichtenberg EM, Hallager S: A description of commonly observed behaviors for the kori bustard (Ardeotis kori). J Ethol. 2008, 26: 17-34. 10.1007/s10164-006-0030-z.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Du Toit J: Giraffe. Mammals of the southern African subregion. Edited by: Skinner JD, Chimimba CT. 2007, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 616-620.

    Google Scholar 

  27. Pratt DM, Anderson VH: Giraffe social behaviour. J Nat Hist. 1985, 19: 771-781. 10.1080/00222938500770471.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Pellew RA: Food consumption and energy budgets of the giraffe. J Appl Ecol. 1984, 21: 141-159. 10.2307/2403043.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Blomqvist PA, Renberg L: Feeding behaviour of Giraffe (Giraffa camelopardalis) in Mokolodi Reserve. 2007, Botswana: University of Uppsala, Minor Field Study, 1653-5634.

    Google Scholar 

  30. Bashaw MJ, Tarou LR, Maki TS, Maple TL: A survey assessment of variables related to stereotypy in captive giraffe and okapi. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2001, 73: 235-247. 10.1016/S0168-1591(01)00137-X.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Du Toit J, Yetman CA: Effects of body size on the diurnal activity budgets of African browsing ruminants. Oecologia. 2007, 143: 327-325.

    Google Scholar 

  32. Cameron EZ, du Toit J: Social influences on vigilance behaviour in giraffes. Giraffa camelopardalis. Anim Behav. 2008, 69: 1337-1344.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Backhaus D: Experimentelle Prüfung des Farbsehvermögens einer Masai-Giraffe (Giraffa camelopardalis tippelskirchi Matschie, 1898). Z Tierpsychol. 1959, 16: 468-477.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Berry PS: Range movements of giraffe in the Luangwa Valley. Zambia. East Afr Wildl J. 1978, 16: 77-83. 10.1111/j.1365-2028.1978.tb00429.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Seeber PA, Ndlovu HT, Duncan P, Ganswindt A: Grazing behaviour of the giraffe in Hwange National Park, Zimbabwe. Afr J Ecol. 2012, 10.1111/j.1365-2028.2011.01314.x.

    Google Scholar 

  36. Langman VA: Giraffe pica behavior and pathology as indicators of nutritional stress. The J Wildl Manag. 1978, 42: 141-147. 10.2307/3800701.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Ciofolo I, Le Pendu Y: The feeding behavior of giraffe in Niger. Mammalia. 2002, 66: 183-194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Kok OB, Opperman DP: Feeding behaviour of the giraffe Giraffa camelopardalis in the Willem-Pretorius-Game-Reserve. Orange-Free-State. S Afr J Wildl Res. 1980, 10: 45-55.

    Google Scholar 

  39. Wyatt JR: Osteophagia in Masai giraffe. East Afr Wildl J. 1971, 9: 157-10.1111/j.1365-2028.1971.tb00232.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Western D: Giraffe chewing a Grant‘s gazelle carcass. Afr J Ecol. 1971, 9: 156-157. 10.1111/j.1365-2028.1971.tb00231.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Kristal MB, Noonan M: Perinatal maternal and neonatal behaviour in the captive reticulated giraffe. S Afr J Zool. 1979, 14: 103-107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Pratt DM, Anderson VH: Giraffe cow-calf relationships and social development of the calf in the Serengeti. Z Tierpsychol. 1979, 51: 233-251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Kristal MB, Noonan M: Note on sleep in captive giraffes (Giraffa camelopardalis reticulata). S Afr J Zool. 1979, 14: 108-

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Dagg AI, Taub A: Flehmen. Mammalia. 1970, 34: 686-695.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Fernandez LT, Bashaw MJ, Sartor RL, Bouwens NR, Maki TS: Tongue twisters: feeding enrichment to reduce oral stereotypy in giraffe. Zoo Biol. 2008, 27: 200-212. 10.1002/zoo.20180.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Sato S, Takagaki I: Tongue-playing in captive giraffe. 22nd International Ethological Conference. 1991, Kyoto: Otani University

    Google Scholar 

  47. Bashaw MJ, Bloomsmith MA, Maple TL, Bercovitch FB: The structure of social relationships among captive female giraffe (Giraffa camelopardalis). J Comp Psychol. 2007, 121: 46-53.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Coe MJ: Necking“ behaviour in the giraffe. J Zool (Lond). 1967, 151: 313-321.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Horwich RH, Kitchen C, Wangel M, Ruthe R: Behavioral development in okapis and giraffes. Zoo Biol. 1983, 2: 105-125. 10.1002/zoo.1430020204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Woods TD: Pre-copulatory behaviour in male giraffe. Lammergeyer. 1972, 17: 67-

    Google Scholar 

  51. Altmann J: Observational study of behaviour: sampling methods. Behavior. 1974, 49: 227-267. 10.1163/156853974X00534.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  52. Loveridge AJ, Searle AW, Murindagamo F, MacDonald DW: The impact of sport-hunting on the population dynamics of an African lion population in a protected area. Biol Conserv. 2007, 134: 548-558. 10.1016/j.biocon.2006.09.010.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Ransom JI, Cade BS: Quantifying equid behavior - a research ethogram for free-roaming feral horses. 2009, U.S: Geological Survey Techniques and Methods 2-A9

    Google Scholar 

  54. Lund-Larsen TR: Relation between testosterone levels in serum and proteolytic activity in the neck muscles of the Norwegian reindeer Rangifer tarandus tarandus. Acta Zool. 1977, 58: 61-63. 10.1111/j.1463-6395.1977.tb00238.x.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  55. Pournelle GH: Notes on the reproduction of a Baringo giraffe. J Mammal. 36: 574-

  56. Childes SL, Walker BH: Ecology and dynamics of the woody vegetation on the Kalahari Sands in Hwange National Park. Zimbabwe. Vegetatio. 1987, 72: 111-128.

    Google Scholar 

  57. Mason GJ: Stereotypies and suffering. Behav Process. 1991, 25: 103-115. 10.1016/0376-6357(91)90013-P.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Carlstead K: Effects of captivity on the behavior of wild mammals. Wild Mammals in Captivity: Principles and Techniques. Edited by: Kleiman DG, Allen ME, Thompson KV, Lumpkin S. 1996, 317-333.

    Google Scholar 

  59. Knappe H: Zur Funktion des Jacobsonschen Organs (Organon vomeronasale Jacobsoni). Zool Gart. 1964, 28: 188-194.

    Google Scholar 

  60. Drews C: The concept and definition of dominance in animal behaviour. Behaviour. 1993, 125: 283-313. 10.1163/156853993X00290.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Boy V, Duncan P: Time-budgets of Camargue horses I. Developmental changes in the time-budgets of foals. Behaviour. 1979, 71: 187-202.

    Google Scholar 

  62. Hirth DH, McCullough DR: Evolution of alarm signals in ungulates with special reference to white-tailed deer. Am Nat. 1977, 111: 31-42. 10.1086/283136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The project was financially supported by the University of Pretoria, the Giraffe Conservation Foundation, the SAVF, and by the CNRS HERD project, directed by H. Fritz. We thank the Zimbabwe Parks and Wildlife Management Authority for permission to conduct research work in Hwange National Park. For observation permission and collaboration we also thank the ecologists team from Entabeni Game Reserve, and the National Zoological Gardens, Pretoria.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Peter A Seeber.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

PAS and AG conceived the study and drafted the manuscript. IC reviewed the initial draft and contributed on information and behavioural interpretation. All authors contributed to, read, and approved the final manuscript.

Electronic supplementary material

13104_2012_2100_MOESM1_ESM.doc

Additional file 1: Table S1: General Activities [2, 4, 9, 16, 20, 21, 23, 2628],[3135, 3852, 5562]. (DOC 6 MB)

13104_2012_2100_MOESM2_ESM.doc

Additional file 2: Table S2: Abnormal repetitive behaviours [16, 20, 26, 30, 45, 46]. (DOC 57 KB)

13104_2012_2100_MOESM3_ESM.doc

Additional file 3: Table S3: General Interactions [5, 18, 19, 23, 27, 42, 44, 4750],[59]. (DOC 7 MB)

13104_2012_2100_MOESM4_ESM.doc

Additional file 4: Table S4: Bull Cow Behaviour [19, 23, 27]. (DOC 4 MB)

13104_2012_2100_MOESM5_ESM.doc

Additional file 5: Table S5: Bull-Bull Behaviour [9, 18, 23, 41, 60]. (DOC 507 KB)

13104_2012_2100_MOESM6_ESM.doc

Additional file 6: Table S6: Cow - Bull Behaviour [23, 27]. (DOC 3 MB)

13104_2012_2100_MOESM7_ESM.doc

Additional file 7: Table S7: Behavioural Interactions by Calves [5, 42, 47]. (DOC 53 KB)

13104_2012_2100_MOESM8_ESM.doc

Additional file 8: Table S8: Maternal behaviour [41, 42, 55]. (DOC 60 KB)

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Seeber, P.A., Ciofolo, I. & Ganswindt, A. Behavioural inventory of the giraffe (Giraffa camelopardalis). BMC Res Notes 5, 650 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1186/1756-0500-5-650

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1756-0500-5-650

Keywords