Skip to main content

Spontaneous rhythms in a harbor seal pup calls

A Correction to this article was published on 11 June 2018

This article has been updated

Abstract

Objectives

Timing and rhythm (i.e. temporal structure) are crucial, though historically neglected, dimensions of animal communication. When investigating these in non-human animals, it is often difficult to balance experimental control and ecological validity. Here I present the first step of an attempt to balance the two, focusing on the timing of vocal rhythms in a harbor seal pup (Phoca vitulina). Collection of this data had a clear aim: To find spontaneous vocal rhythms in this individual in order to design individually-adapted and ecologically-relevant stimuli for a later playback experiment.

Data description

The calls of one seal pup were recorded. The audio recordings were annotated using Praat, a free software to analyze vocalizations in humans and other animals. The annotated onsets and offsets of vocalizations were then imported in a Python script. The script extracted three types of timing information: the duration of calls, the intervals between calls’ onsets, and the intervals between calls’ maximum-intensity peaks. Based on the annotated data, available to download, I provide simple descriptive statistics for these temporal measures, and compare their distributions.

Objective

Pinnipeds are a clade of marine mammals exhibiting a range of vocal behaviors [1, 2]. Testing rhythm and timing in pinnipeds is relevant to a number of cross-species evolutionary hypotheses relating rhythmic behaviors to vocal flexibility, social cognition, and brain plasticity [3,4,5,6,7,8].

Traditionally, the comparative study of rhythm and timing has spanned two main strands. Animal vocalizations and behaviors have been recorded in ecologically-relevant settings to unveil temporal structures. These observational approaches allowed little experimental control. Alternatively, animals have been tested in an operant setup, employing controlled external stimuli to trigger animals’ reactions. These other approaches traded ecological relevance for experimental control. While a few exceptions exist [9], animal research on rhythm and timing still needs to strike a good tradeoff between experimental rigor and ecological relevance.

Here I present the first step of an attempt to balance the two. I describe and share data on spontaneous vocal rhythms in a harbor seal pup. Audio recordings were collected with the explicit aim of finding the natural timing of vocal production in this individual, and design ecologically-relevant and individually-adapted stimuli for a future playback experiment [9,10,11].

After recording the animal, I annotated the onsets and offsets of vocalizations in Praat. Using a custom Python script (Table 1, Data file 1) [12, 13], I extracted three sorts of timing information: the duration of calls [14,15,16,17], the intervals between calls’ onsets [18, 19], and the intervals between calls’ maximum-intensity peaks [20].

Table 1 Overview of data files/data sets

Data description

Subject

I recorded a female harbor seal pup. The seal was born in the wild and brought into rehabilitation at the Sealcentre Pieterburen, The Netherlands [21, 22], at the estimated age of 7 days [14, 22]. The animal was individually housed in a pool situated in a 1-room cabin. Seals in rehabilitation are usually housed in pairs [14]; this recording exploited the rare occurrence of individual housing.

Sound recordings

On the twenty-first day from estimated birth, 10 min of vocalizations were recorded in air using a unidirectional microphone Sennheiser ME-66 (frequency response: 40–20,000 Hz ± 2.5 dB; Sennheiser electronic GmbH&Co. KG, Wedemark, Germany) [14]. The microphone was equipped with a MZW-66 foam windshield, and was connected to a digital recorder Zoom H6 (Zoom Corporation, Tokyo, Japan). Recordings, collected at 0.5–2 m distance from the seal, were saved as a .wav file (48 kHz sampling frequency; 24-bit quantization).

Call annotations

The audio file was manually annotated in Praat version 6.0.11 [23]. Mother attraction calls (MACs) and other calls were annotated as two different categories on one tier. The tier was saved as a .TextGrid file. Only clear MACs [10, 11, 17, 24] were retained for further computations [14, 15, 17].

Extraction of temporal variables

A Python 2.7 script extracted and combined annotations and sound features (Table 1, Data file 1), and outputted five .csv files. The script imported the annotations using package TextGridTools 1.4.3 [12] and the wave sound using Parselmouth [13]. The script calculated: durations (Table 1, Data file 2), inter-onset intervals (IOIs), and inter-peak intervals (IPIs) of calls. An IOI was defined as the time elapsed between the onsets of two consecutive calls (Table 1, Data file 3). An IPI was defined as the time between the maximum-intensity peaks of two consecutive calls (Table 1, Data file 4) [20]. Two more datasets were computed and output: short IOIs (IOIs, Table 1, Data file 5) and short IPIs (IPIs, Table 1, Data file 6), consisting of intervals within approximately 4 times the minimum value (≈ 3900 ms). The purpose of this threshold was to focus on timing within vocalization bouts (IOIs and IPIs) as opposed to pooled timing within and between bouts (IOI and IPI).

Descriptive statistics

Mean call duration was 976.1 ms (standard deviation σ = 205.7, see also Table 1, Data file 7). Mean IOI was 8578.3 ms (σ = 7807.4). Mean IPI was 8574.6 ms (σ = 7839.8). No significant difference was detected between these two distributions (Two-sample Kolmogorov–Smirnov test, D = 0.04, p = 0.99). Mean IOIs was 1983.2 ms (σ = 722.1). Mean IPIs was 2020.8 ms (σ = 803.3). No significant difference was detected between distributions of IOIss and IPIss (Two-sample Kolmogorov–Smirnov test, D = 0.10, p = 0.99). In other words, using onsets instead of peaks does not yield a significant difference between distributions. This holds at two different timescales, i.e. for both the IOI/IPI and the IOIs/IPIs comparisons. The distributions of IOI and IPI have very high σ, almost equal to their means (CV, coefficient of variation, equals 0.91 for IOI and IPI). Conversely, the distributions of IOIs and IPIs have lower σ (CV equals 0.36 for IOIs and 0.39 for IPIs).

Limitations

A clear limitation of these data is their focus on one individual. Pups in rehabilitation are usually kept in groups. Hence, it is uncommon to record long runs of vocalizations from isolated individuals. Data from this individual seal served its purpose of tailoring an experiment to her [25]. However, solid inference about rhythm ontogeny, learning, individual differences, and species differences will require additional data [7]. It would be desirable to collect a panel dataset, where multiple animals are recorded daily, showing variance over individuals and time. Such dataset would enable comparing the (1) type of temporal distributions, (2) average length, and (3) degree of isochronous regularity, both between species (e.g. Phocids vs. Otariids [6, 7, 18]) and between housing conditions (lonely vs. in-pair housing). Seal pups’ call duration is known to vary with age [14, 15, 17]; all the other temporal variables presented in this Data Note are rarely investigated in pinniped bioacoustics.

A second limitation is that vocalizations were exclusively recorded in-air, while harbor seal pups [17] and adults [16] also vocalize underwater. This might not be an issue, because the medium of sound transmission should affect spectral, rather than temporal, properties of the calls [6]. However, vocal production repertoires might also vary across media, with some vocalizations only appearing in-air or underwater. Past research found that call duration is comparable across media [17]; comparison of IOIs and IPIs across media remains, to my knowledge, unexplored.

To comply with Data Note articles’ guidelines, this paper lacks analyses. Although simple descriptive statistics are suited for the original purpose of these recordings, namely estimating the mean IOIs and using it to produce experimental stimuli, some analyses could be performed [19, 26].

Change history

  • 11 June 2018

    Following publication of the original article [1], the author reported an error in the first sentence of the ‘Sound recordings’ section. The recordings mentioned in this sentence were performed 6 days later than written. In this Correction article the original and corrected version of the sentence are presented.

Abbreviations

MAC:

mother attraction calls

IOI:

inter-onset interval

IOIs :

short inter-onset interval

IPI:

inter-peak interval

IPIs :

short inter-peak interval

σ:

one standard deviation from the mean

CV:

coefficient of variation

References

  1. Reichmuth C, Casey C. Vocal learning in seals, sea lions, and walruses. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2014;28:66–71.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  2. Schusterman R, Southall B, Kastak D, Reichmuth Kastak C. Pinniped vocal communication: Form and function. In: Proceedings of the 17th International Congress on Acoustics, Rome, Italy; 2001. p. 1–2.

  3. Cook P, Rouse A, Wilson M, Reichmuth CJ. A California Sea Lion (Zalophus californianus) can keep the beat: motor entrainment to rhythmic auditory stimuli in a non vocal mimic. J Comp Psychol. 2013;127(2):1–16.

    Google Scholar 

  4. Patel AD. Musical rhythm, linguistic rhythm, and human evolution. Music Percept Interdiscip J. 2006;24(1):99–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Patel AD. The evolutionary biology of musical rhythm: was Darwin wrong? PLoS Biol. 2014;12(3):e1001821.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  6. Ravignani A. Comment on “Temporal and spatial variation in harbor seal (Phoca vitulina L.) roar calls from southern Scandinavia” [J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 141, 1824-1834 (2017)]. J Acoust Soc Am. (in review).

  7. Ravignani A, Fitch WT, Hanke FD, Heinrich T, Hurgitsch B, Kotz SA, Scharff C, Stoeger AS, de Boer B. What pinnipeds have to say about human speech, music, and the evolution of rhythm. Front Neurosci. 2016;10(274):1–9.

    Google Scholar 

  8. Ralls K, Fiorelli P, Gish S. Vocalizations and vocal mimicry in captive harbor seals, Phoca vitulina. Can J Zool. 1985;63(5):1050–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Mathevon N, Casey C, Reichmuth C, Charrier I. Northern elephant seals memorize the rhythm and timbre of their rivals’ voices. Curr Biol. 2017;27(15):2352–6.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. Perry EA, Renouf D. Further studies of the role of harbour seal (Phoca vitulina) pup vocalizations in preventing separation of mother–pup pairs. Can J Zool. 1988;66(4):934–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Sauvé CC, Beauplet G, Hammill MO, Charrier I. Mother–pup vocal recognition in harbour seals: influence of maternal behaviour, pup voice and habitat sound properties. Anim Behav. 2015;105:109–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Buschmeier H, Wlodarczak M. TextGridTools: A TextGrid processing and analysis toolkit for Python. In: Proceedings der 27 Konferenz zur Elektronischen Sprachsignalverarbeitung. Bielefeld; 2013. p. 152–7.

  13. Jadoul Y, Thompson B, de Boer B. Introducing Parselmouth: a Python interface to Praat. J Phon (in review).

  14. de Reus K. Talking seals: vocal development in Eastern Atlantic harbour seal pups (Phoca vitulina vitulina). M.Sc. Royal Veterinary College, University of London; 2017.

  15. Khan CB, Markowitz H, McCowan B. Vocal development in captive harbor seal pups, Phoca vitulina richardii: age, sex, and individual differences. J Acoust Soc Am. 2006;120(3):1684–94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Sabinsky PF, Larsen ON, Wahlberg M, Tougaard J. Temporal and spatial variation in harbor seal (Phoca vitulina L.) roar calls from southern Scandinavia. J Acoust Soc Am. 2017;141(3):1824–34.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Sauvé CC, Beauplet G, Hammill MO, Charrier I. Acoustic analysis of airborne, underwater, and amphibious mother attraction calls by wild harbor seal pups (Phoca vitulina). J Mammal. 2015;96(3):591–602.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Ravignani A, Madison G. The paradox of isochrony in the evolution of human rhythm. Front Psychol. 1820;2017(8):1–13.

    Google Scholar 

  19. Ravignani A, Norton P. Measuring rhythmic complexity: a primer to quantify and compare temporal structure in speech, movement, and animal vocalizations. J Lang Evol. 2017;2(1):4–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Jadoul Y, Ravignani A, Thompson B, Filippi P, de Boer B. Seeking temporal predictability in speech: comparing statistical approaches on 18 world languages. Front Hum Neurosci. 2016;10(586):1–15.

    Google Scholar 

  21. Osinga N, Pen I, de Haes HU, Brakefield P. Evidence for a progressively earlier pupping season of the common seal (Phoca vitulina) in the Wadden Sea. J Mar Biol Assoc UK. 2012;92(08):1663–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Ravignani A, Gross S, Garcia M, Rubio-Garcia A, de Boer B. How small could a pup sound? The physical bases of signaling body size in harbor seals. Curr Zool. 2017;63(4):457–65.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Boersma, Weenink. 2017. http://www.praat.org/. Accessed 1 Jan 2017.

  24. Renouf D. The vocalization of the harbour seal pup (Phoca vitulina) and its role in the maintenance of contact with the mother. J Zool. 1984;202(4):583–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Ravignani A. A selfish seal chorus: timing of contagious calling in a harbor seal pup (in preparation).

  26. Kello CT, Dalla Bella S, Médé B, Balasubramaniam R. Hierarchical temporal structure in music, speech and animal vocalizations: jazz is like a conversation, humpbacks sing like hermit thrushes. J R Soc Interface. 2017;14(135):20170231.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The author is grateful to Peter Cook, Bart de Boer, Koen de Reus, Sonja Kotz, Marga Mendez-Arostegui, Ana Rubio-Garcia, Marianna Anichini, and Ruth Sonnweber for advice on the research and/or manuscript.

Competing interests

The author declares that he has no competing interests.

Availability of data materials

The data described in this Data note can be freely and openly accessed on Figshare https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.5616490. Please see Table 1 and reference list for details and links to the data.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The audio recordings were non-invasive and did not require ethical approval. Data collection was in accordance with Belgian, Dutch, and European Union legislation, and the guidelines of the Association for the Study of Animal Behavior.

Funding

AR has received funding from the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme under the Marie Skłodowska-Curie Grant Agreement No. 665501 with the research Foundation Flanders (FWO) (Pegasus2 Marie Curie fellowship 12N5517N), and a visiting fellowship in Language Evolution from the Max Planck Society.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Andrea Ravignani.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Ravignani, A. Spontaneous rhythms in a harbor seal pup calls. BMC Res Notes 11, 3 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13104-017-3107-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13104-017-3107-6

Keywords