Skip to main content

Male size, not female preferences influence female reproductive success in a poeciliid fish (Poecilia latipinna): a combined behavioural/genetic approach

Abstract

Objective

We investigated the potential role of indirect benefits for female mate preferences in a highly promiscuous species of live-bearing fishes, the sailfin molly Poecilia latipinna using an integrative approach that combines methods from animal behavior, life-history evolution, and genetics. Males of this species solely contribute sperm for reproduction, and consequently females do not receive any direct benefits. Despite this, females typically show clear mate preferences. It has been suggested that females can increase their reproductive success through indirect benefits from choosing males of higher quality.

Results

Although preferences for large body size have been recorded as an honest signal for genetic quality, this particular study resulted in female preference being unaffected by male body size. Nonetheless, larger males did sire more offspring, but with no effect on offspring quality. This study presents a methodical innovation by combining preference testing with life history measurements—such as the determination of the dry weight of fish embryos—and paternity analyses on single fish embryos.

Introduction

Female mate choice is one of the key drivers of sexual selection [1, 2]. In species where males provide direct benefits to females, this is easily understood. However, in many species such direct benefits are not apparent, yet female choice exists despite clear costs, such as, increased risk of predation, loss of time, and energetic costs [3,4,5,6,7,8]. To balance the costs of choice, indirect (genetic) benefits have been invoked. They are much harder to document, but are thought to enhance female fitness through an increased reproductive value of the offspring [1, 2, 6, 9]. Despite the immense theoretical support and conceptual relevance of the evolution of female choice for indirect benefits, it still remains controversial how important those effects are (meta-analysis by [10]).

Here, we studied the evolution of female choice for indirect benefits by investigating female mating preference of the sailfin molly, Poecilia latipinna. The sailfin molly is a particularly well-suited model species to test female choice for indirect benefits because it is a sexually reproducing and promiscuous species. Males only contribute ejaculates (mainly sperm) to reproduction without providing any direct benefits. Yet, females typically show clear mate preferences, e.g. for large male body size [11, 12]. In the present study, we assessed female preference for two males of different size, which we used as a proxy for male quality. Body size is heritable [13,14,15,16,17], and larger offspring generally have higher fitness in many different taxa [18,19,20,21,22], including fishes [23,24,25]. After determining female preferences, in our experiment, females were allowed to mate with the two males, a large and a small one. We measured both offspring quantity (number of embryos) and a proxy of offspring quality (embryo dry weight). We investigated three hypotheses: (I) we expected females to prefer larger, higher quality males, (II) we expected larger and preferred males to sire more offspring, (III) and finally, we expected larger and preferred males to sire offspring of higher quality. Furthermore, we wanted to test if it would be possible to utilize ethanol preserved fish embryos for both DNA paternity testing using microsatellites and assessment of life history data (i.e., dry weight after gentle low temperature desiccation). The usage of microsatellite markers is a widely accepted and reliable method to assess parentage (reviewed in [26]). Embryo body weight is an important life history measure, as larger offspring generally have a higher fitness (see above).

Main text

Methods

Fish collection and maintenance

Fish were collected in August 2013 in Brownsville, Texas (Rio Grande Valley, Cameron County; 25°54.58′N 97°26.61′W), P. latipinna (approx. 200–300 individuals; including males and females) were caught using a standard 1.2 m seine, with 0.3 cm mesh width, and transported to the University of Oklahoma in Norman, Oklahoma. Males (N = 60) were held in a common tank (60 L). Gravid females (N = 42) were isolated and kept in individual tanks (7 L). All fish were maintained under standardised conditions (24–28 °C water temperature, aerated and filtered water, weekly water changes, specific conductance (SPC) of the water: 1300–1800 μS/cm, 12:12 h light:dark cycle). Twice daily the fish were fed ad libitum with flake food in the mornings and frozen mosquito larvae in the evenings.

Female mate preference

A detailed description of the procedure for female preference assessment can be found in Additional file 1. Females (N = 27) were tested for their mate preference using binary choice tests (standard procedure for testing preferences; [27,28,29]), as soon as possible after giving birth to a brood (time lag 5–36 h). After parturition, females can be readily fertilised by males for approximately 3–4 days [30, 31]. It was assumed that female choice would be most acute during this phase. Females were allowed to choose between one small (mean ± SE = 29.5 ± 0.3 mm) and one large male (mean ± SE = 34.1 ± 0.6 mm; size difference within male pairs: mean ± SD = 5.0 ± 0.1 mm). Each male pair was only used once. We quantified female preference as the time spent with a male relative to the time spent with both males (please see Additional file 1). The two males presented to a female were defined as the preferred (preference > 50%; mean ± SE = 65.17 ± 10.58%) and the non-preferred male (preference < 50%; mean ± SE = 34.83 ± 10.58%). All males (mean ± SE = 31.8 ± 0.5 cm) and females (mean ± SE = 45.1 ± 0.6 cm) were measured for standard length after mate choice trials.

Mating

Immediately after the choice test, females were mated with their preferred and non-preferred male. Mating order was randomized. One of the two males was placed in the female’s tank for 24 h; followed by another 24 h with the other male. Mating experiments were performed in small 7 L tanks to increase the likelihood of a mating through a higher interaction rate. Half of the females were mated with the preferred male first (N = 13; with 8 large and 5 small males) and half were mated with the non-preferred male first (N = 14, 7 large and 7 small males). This was repeated, so that the overall mating period amounted to 4 days, spanning the entirety of the female’s fertile phase. Because not all females preferred the large male, we were able to cross male size and mating order in this experiment. After the mating period, a small dorsal fin-clip of each male was collected and preserved in 100% ethanol to yield paternal DNA. Twenty-two days after the mating ended, females were euthanized using an overdose of Tricaine methanesulfonate (MS-222) and preserved in 100% ethanol until dissection. Females were euthanized before they could release their brood. A period of 22 days was chosen because gestation of poeciliid fishes takes approximately 30 days [30, 31].

Life history measures and paternity analysis

Prior to dissection, a small fin-clip was taken of each female to provide maternal DNA. Females were dissected following the protocol of Riesch et al. [32]. The developmental stage of each embryo was classified following the protocol of Riesch et al. [33]. During dissection and classification, embryos were kept in 100% ethanol and stored separately to prevent degradation and contamination of DNA. Embryos were dried in an incubator oven for 10 days at 40 °C [33] and then placed in a desiccator for 3–4 h to remove residual dampness before embryo dry weight (mg) was measured.

If available, a subset of 20 embryos per female was randomly chosen as representatives for the whole brood (N = 110, 15–20 embryos per female). For the paternity analysis, embryos and potential parents were genotyped at 10 unlinked polymorphic microsatellite loci [34], please see Additional file 2.

Data analysis

Statistical analyses were performed using R version 3.2 [35]. We tested for a difference in female preference (arcsine-square root-transformed) of small versus large males using a paired t-test (N = 27). A t-test power analysis was computed using the R package pwr [36]. Further, we fitted a linear regression model (LM) on female preference for large males (N = 27; arcsine-square root-transformed) using male size and the size difference between the small and large male within a pair as predictors. We analysed offspring quantity by fitting an LM on the number of offspring sired by each male (N = 6). The number of offspring sired by a male equaled a female’s fecundity (i.e., we did not detect multiple paternity). The full model incorporated female preference and size for the father as predictor variables. Offspring quality was analysed by fitting a linear mixed-effect model implemented in the lme4-package [37] on the embryo dry weight (N = 110 embryos originating from 6 females), including female preference and size of the father as the fixed effects and mother ID as a random effect. For all models, non-significant predictors were eliminated in a stepwise backward procedure. Before analyses predictors were z-transformed (except for the developmental stage) to control for potential non-linear relationships between variables. Prior to generating z-scores, predictors were transformed: number of offspring: square root-transformation; female preference: arcsine-square root-transformation; embryo dry weight: x2-transformation; male and female size: log10-transformation; [38]. Using preparatory linear regression analyses, we removed the effect of female size on offspring number and the effect of the developmental stage and female size on the embryo dry weight before the analyses. Our data did not show deviations from a normal distribution (Shapiro–Wilk-tests).

Results

We found no significant difference in female preference for small (mean ± SE = 45.29 ± 3.48%) vs. large males (mean ± SE = 54.70 ± 3.48%), (paired t-test; t26 = 1.3691, P = 0.1827, power = 0.7316; Fig. 1). Female preference did neither correlate with male size (linear regression: F1, 25 = 0.0256, P = 0.8742) nor with the size difference between males (linear regression: F1, 25 = 0.9348, P = 0.3429), for female preference data please see Additional file 3.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Female preference for male standard length did not deviate from random choice (50%; dashed line). Box plots with 1.5 interquartile range, mean (white diamond), and median (−); ns non-significant

The number of offspring sired by a male was significantly influenced by male size (LM; F1,4 = 8.0365, P = 0.0471, adjusted R2 = 0.5846; Fig. 2). However, this result is based on a low sample size of only six females with offspring (see Additional file 3). Female preference for the father (mean ± SE = 55.48 ± 11.18%), (LM; F1,3 = 0.5421, P = 0.5149) did not affect the number of offspring. All offspring were exclusively sired by the larger male, independent of the female’s behavioural preference (3 females preferred the large male, 3 females preferred the small male), data are given in Additional file 4. Furthermore, there was a strong effect of the mating order on offspring production. Only first males that were large sired offspring. Offspring dry weight was not influenced by male size (LMM; \(\chi^{2}_{1}\) = 0.2311, P = 0.6307). Also, female preference did not influence the dry weight (LMM; \(\chi^{2}_{1}\) = 0.0002, P = 0.9901).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Positive relation between male size and number of offspring sired. Original data presented (N = 6)

Discussion

Female mating preferences for large male body size are widespread within a broad range of taxa (review by: [39]), with P. latipinna being no exception [11, 12, 28, 40, 41], but such a preference was not detected in the present study. Female preferences are based on the recognition of multiple cues, e.g. colouration [42] courtship behaviour [43] or MHC similarity [44,45,46], which provides a much more powerful assessment of male quality when combined than size does alone. In this study, females received additional visual, behavioural and olfactory cues during mate choice trials, which might have interfered with female preference for large male body size.

Only a subset of females had offspring and due to the low sample size we are interpreting our findings with great caution. Interestingly, however, although male size was not important in determining female preferences, it had an effect on the number of offspring sired by a male. Offspring were sired only by large males and offspring quantity increased with male size, providing evidence that male intrinsic quality may increase female reproductive success. A potential mechanism would be a positive correlation of male size with quality or quantity of sperm. Male size has been shown to correlate with the amount of sperm in the eastern mosquitofish, Gambusia holbrooki [47], mandarinfish, Synchiropus splendidus [48] and P. latipinna [49]. Furthermore, mating order had an effect on male reproductive success with only first males siring offspring indicating first-male precedence. Effects of insemination order on fertilization success are widely distributed in animals with internal fertilization [50, 51]. As the effect of male size and mating order were confounded, it is unclear whether first male precedence or male size per se generated this pattern.

Although offspring quantity was influenced by male size, offspring quality (measured as embryo dry weight) was not. Offspring quality is determined by various components, and to obtain substantial insight into the influence of mate preferences and mate quality on reproductive success, future studies need to consider various aspects of offspring quality, including offspring performance.

Limitations

Clearly, our study is limited by the low number of successful matings, resulting in a small final sample size. Also, fish were collected at a single collection site not allowing to control for population effects.

References

  1. Andersson M. Sexual selection. Princeton: Princeton University Press; 1994.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Jones AG, Ratterman NL. Mate choice and sexual selection: what have we learned since Darwin? Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2009;106:10001–8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Pomiankowski A. The costs of choice in sexual selection. J Theor Biol. 1987;128:195–218.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. Crowley PH, Travers SE, Linton MC, Cohn SL, Sih A, Sargent RC. Predation risk, and the seasonal sequence of mate choices: a dynamic game. Am Nat. 1991;137:567–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Kokko H, Mappes J. Sexual selection when fertilization is not guaranteed. Evolution. 2005;59:1876–85.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Kokko H, Brooks R, Jennions MD, Morley J. The evolution of mate choice and mating biases. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 2003;270:653–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Bleu J, Bessa-Gomes C, Laloi D. Evolution of female choosiness and mating frequency: effects of mating cost, density and sex ratio. Anim Behav. 2012;83:131–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Etienne L, Rousset F, Godelle B, Courtiol A. How choosy should I be? The relative searching time predicts evolution of choosiness under direct sexual selection. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 2014;281:20140190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Kuijper B, Pen I, Weissing FJ. A guide to sexual selection theory. Annu Rev Ecol Evol Syst. 2012;43:287–311.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Møller AP, Alatalo RV. Good-genes effects in sexual selection. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1999;266:85–91.

    Google Scholar 

  11. Marler CA, Ryan MJ. Origin and maintenance of a female mating preference. Evolution. 1997;51:1244–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Ptacek MB, Travis J. Mate choice in the sailfin molly, Poecilia latipinna. Evolution. 1997;51:1217–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Campton DE. Heritability of body size of green swordtails, Xiphophorus-helleri.1. Sib analyses of males reared individually and in groups. J Hered. 1992;83:43–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Day TH, Crean CS, Gilburn AS, Shuker DM, Wilcockson RW. Sexual selection in seaweed flies: genetic variation in male size and its reliability as an indicator in natural populations. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 1996;263:1127–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Gunay F, Alten B, Ozsoy ED. Narrow-sense heritability of body size and its response to different developmental temperatures in Culex quinquefasciatus (Say 1923). J Vector Ecol. 2011;36:348–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Mook-Kanamori DO, van Beijsterveldt CEM, Steegers EAP, Aulchenko YS, Raat H, Hofman A, Eilers PH, Boomsma DI, Jaddoe VWV. Heritability estimates of body size in fetal life and early childhood. PLoS ONE. 2012;7:e39901.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  17. Thériault V, Garant D, Bernatchez L, Dodson JJ. Heritability of life-history tactics and genetic correlation with body size in a natural population of brook charr (Salvelinus fontinalis). J Evol Biol. 2007;20:2266–77.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Tessier AJ, Consolatti NL. Variation in offspring size in Daphnia and consequences for individual fitness. Oikos. 1989;56:269–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Marshall D, Cook CN, Emlet RB. Offspring size effects mediate competitive interactions in a colonial marine invertebrate. Ecology. 2006;87:214–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Altwegg R, Reyer H-u. Patterns of natural selection on size at metamorphosis in water frogs. Evolution. 2003;57:872–82.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Krist M. Egg size and offspring quality: a meta-analysis in birds. Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc. 2011;86:692–716.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Janzen FJ, Tucker JK, Paukstis GL. Experimental analysis of an early life-history stage: avian predation selects for larger body size of hatchling turtles. J Evol Biol. 2000;13:947–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Einum S, Fleming IA. Maternal effects of egg size in brown trout (Salmo trutta): norms of reaction to environmental quality. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 1999;266:2095–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Hutchings JA. Fitness consequences of variation in egg size and food abundance in brook trout Salvelinus fontinalis. Evolution. 1991;45:1162–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Reynolds JD, Gross MR. Female mate preference enhances offspring growth and reproduction in a fish, Poecilia reticulata. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 1992;250:57–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Jones AG, Ardren WR. Methods of parentage analysis in natural populations. Mol Ecol. 2003;12:2511–23.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  27. Gabor CR, Aspbury AS. Non-repeatable mate choice by male sailfin mollies, Poecilia latipinna, in a unisexual-bisexual mating complex. Behav Ecol. 2008;19:871–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Schlupp I, Marler C, Ryan MJ. Benefit to male sailfin mollies of mating with heterospecific females. Science. 1994;263:373–4.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  29. Schlüter A, Parzefall J, Schlupp I. Female preference for symmetrical vertical bars in male sailfin mollies. Anim Behav. 1998;56:147–53.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Farr JA, Travis J. Fertility advertisement by female sailfin mollies, Poecilia latipinna (Pisces, Poeciliidae). Copeia. 1986;2:467–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Snelson FF, Wetherington JD, Large HL. The relationship between interbrood interval and yolk loading in a generalized poeciliid fish, Poecilia latipinna. Copeia. 1986;2:295–304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Riesch R, Plath M, Schlupp I. Toxic hydrogen sulfide and dark caves: life-history adaptations in a livebearing fish (Poecilia mexicana, Poeciliidae). Ecology. 2010;91:1494–505.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Riesch R, Schlupp I, Langerhans RB, Plath M. Shared and unique patterns of embryo development in extremophile poeciliids. PLoS ONE. 2011;6:e27377.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  34. Tiedemann R, Moll K, Paulus KB, Schlupp I. New microsatellite loci confirm hybrid origin, parthenogenetic inheritance, and mitotic gene conversion in the gynogenetic Amazon molly (Poecilia formosa). Mol Ecol Notes. 2005;5:586–9.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  35. R Core Team. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2015.

    Google Scholar 

  36. Champely S. pwr: Basic functions for power analysis. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=pwr R package version 1.1–3; 2015.

  37. Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B, Walker S. lme4: linear mixed-effects models using Eigen and S4. J Stat Softw. 2014;67:1–48.

    Google Scholar 

  38. Riesch R, Plath M, García de León FJ, Schlupp I. Convergent life-history shifts: toxic environments result in big babies in two clades of poeciliids. Naturwissenschaften. 2010;97:133–41.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  39. Ryan MJ, Keddy-Hector A. Directional pattern of female mate choice and the role of sensory biases. Am Nat. 1992;139:S4–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. MacLaren RD, Rowland WJ, Behaviour S, Mar N. Differences in female preference for male body size in Poecilia latipinna using simultaneous versus sequential stimulus presentation designs. Behaviour. 2006;143:273–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. MacLaren RD, Rowland WJ, Morgan N. Female preferences for sailfin and body size in the sailfin molly, Poecilia latipinna. Ethology. 2004;110:363–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Fukuda S, Karino K. Male red coloration, female mate preference, and sperm longevity in the cyprinid fish Puntius titteya. Environ Biol Fishes. 2014;97:1197–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Kodric-Brown A. Female choice of multiple male criteria in guppies: interacting effects coloration and courtship of dominance. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 1993;32:415–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Aeschlimann PB, Häberli MA, Reusch TBH, Boehm T, Milinski M. Female sticklebacks Gasterosteus aculeatus use self-reference to optimize MHC allele number during mate selection. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2003;54:119–26.

    Google Scholar 

  45. Forsberg LA, Dannewitz J, Petersson E, Grahn M. Influence of genetic dissimilarity in the reproductive success and mate choice of brown trout—females fishing for optimal MHC dissimilarity. J Evol Biol. 2007;20:1859–69.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  46. Landry C, Garant D, Duchesne P, Bernatchez L. ‘Good genes as heterozygosity’: the major histocompatibility complex and mate choice in Atlantic salmon (Salmo salar). Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 2001;268:1279–85.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  47. O’Dea RE, Jennions MD, Head ML. Male body size and condition affects sperm number and production rates in mosquitofish, Gambusia holbrooki. J Evol Biol. 2014;27:2739–44.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Rasotto MB, De Mitcheson YS, Mitcheson G. Male body size predicts sperm number in the mandarinfish. J Zool. 2010;281:161–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Schlupp I, Plath M. Male mate choice and sperm allocation in a sexual/asexual mating complex of Poecilia (Poeciliidae, Teleostei). Biol Lett. 2005;1:169–71.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  50. Magris M, Cardozo G, Santi F, Devigili A, Pilastro A. Artificial insemination unveils a first-male fertilization advantage in the guppy. Anim Behav. 2017;131:45–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Jones AG, Adams EM, Arnold SJ. Topping off: a mechanism of first-male sperm precedence in a vertebrate. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2002;99:2078–81.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors’ contributions

Concept and experimental set-up was designed by IS and US. RT designed and supervised the genetic paternity analysis. US was responsible of data acquisition and analysis. IS, RT and US were involved in drafting and revising the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank C. Burt and A. Makowicz for their assistance in designing the experiment and their contributions to the manuscript. T. McDonald also provided valuable comments on the manuscript. Thanks to L. Arriaga for his help with fish maintenance and K. Havenstein for her assistance during lab work. The study was conducted in collaboration of the University of Oklahoma and the University of Potsdam, both of which kindly provided facilities and financial support. Further, we thank an anonymous reviewer and an editorial advisor for their constructive comments on the manuscript. We acknowledge the support of the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft and Open Access Publishing Fund of University of Potsdam.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Availability of data and materials

All data presented and analysed are given in Additional files 3 and 4. Further information available upon request.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

This work was approved by the University of Oklahoma Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee. The state of Texas provided a scientific collecting permit.

Funding

US was financially supported by DAAD (Deutscher Akademischer Austauschdienst).

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ralph Tiedemann.

Additional files

Additional file 1.

Binary choice test. Detailed description for the determination of female preference.

Additional file 2.

Paternity analysis. Detailed description for the determination of paternity.

Additional file 3.

Data on female mate choice, mating and fecundity.

Additional file 4.

Results paternity analysis and offspring life history data.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Scherer, U., Tiedemann, R. & Schlupp, I. Male size, not female preferences influence female reproductive success in a poeciliid fish (Poecilia latipinna): a combined behavioural/genetic approach. BMC Res Notes 11, 364 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13104-018-3487-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13104-018-3487-2

Keywords