Skip to main content

Deficits in visuo-spatial but not in topographical memory during pregnancy and the postpartum state in an expert military pilot: a case report

Abstract

Background

It is well known that cognitive and emotional changes occur during pregnancy, but little is known about their magnitude or their time of occurrence and recovery. During pregnancy memory is one of the most impaired cognitive functions. Although long-term aspects of memory have been investigated, other aspects of memory have not yet been explored (i.e., navigational memory and reaching memory).

Case presentation

Here we describe the changes in reaching and walking memory that occurred during pregnancy and one year after delivery in an Italian female military pilot (Case 1) with high spatial ability. In memory tests she showed a classical dissociation between performance in reaching and walking distance, which indicated a failure of working memory, learning, and storage in reaching space. This suggests that her expertise served as a protective factor mitigating her low walking memory performance, and saving the topographical component.

We compared her performance with that of two non-pregnant control groups (i.e., women pilots and non-pilots) and found that Case 1’s reaching memory performance was significantly worse than that of the control groups. Even one year postpartum, Case 1’s performance was not yet the same as that of the other pilots.

Conclusions

These findings contribute to our knowledge of the specific, as yet unexplored, aspects of memory deficits in women pilots during pregnancy and postpartum and suggest the need for better neuropsychological assessment before these women return to work in operational environments.

Background

Recently, a great deal of attention has been paid to cognitive changes during pregnancy and postpartum. Previously, Poser et al. [1] observed that over 80% of pregnant women reported increased forgetfulness. In a review of the literature on this topic, Brett and Baxendale [2] concluded that both forgetfulness and poor memory were top on the list of difficulties mentioned by pregnant women. In fact, several studies found that retrospective memory was adversely affected by pregnancy (e.g., see [24]) but other studies did not (see [57], and for a review, see [8]). Until now, studies of the memory changes that occur at different stages of pregnancy and at different times postpartum have investigated aspects of memory which vary with regard to temporal duration (working memory and long-term memory) and storage capacity. However, no deficits have been reported between pregnant and postpartum women regarding the storage component of working memory [9, 10]. Some studies found that pregnancy selectively affected the retrieval component of long-term memory, specifically in free recall [3, 4, 11]. Explicit and implicit memory have also been explored. Explicit memory requires the deliberate recall of information from a specific learning episode and implicit memory refers to the unconscious accessing of previous experiences in the absence of intentional recollection. Sharp et al. [12] found that irrespective of gravida status or trimester of pregnancy, pregnant women were significantly impaired on measures of both types of memory. More recently, however, McDowall and Moriarty [7] failed to identify any significant differences between pregnant and non-pregnant women on measures of implicit and explicit memory. In Henry and Rendell ‘s [8] meta-analytic study of memory changes in pregnancy it emerged that deficits were present only in tasks with high demands on effortful processing and, specifically, with measures of free recall and tasks involving the executive component of working memory. Henry and Rendell [8] also observed that the pattern of deficits observed postpartum mirrors the pattern of deficits observed during pregnancy both qualitatively and quantitatively. Furthermore, they found that women accurately estimated the quality of their performance; in fact, the effect sizes for self-reported memory deficits were similar in magnitude to the effect sizes observed for the behavioural measures [8]. Note that the magnitude of the deficits was relatively subtle, which might partially explain why different studies failed to find homogeneous results. Indeed, the results might depend on the sensitivity of the instrument used to detect the memory deficits. To our knowledge, no findings have been reported about memory in reaching and walking distance during pregnancy and postpartum.

The space around us is a multifactorial construct of our brain and distinct areas are responsible for coding space that is behaviourally defined as outside reaching distance (far space) and as within reaching distance (near space) (e.g., [13]). The coding of space as near and far is not only determined by arm-reaching distance, but also depends on how the brain represents the extension of the body space. There is an evolutionary reason for this distinction. Near space, also called reaching space, refers to the portion of space within “grasping distance” (i.e. the space in which a seated individual can grasp an object), whereas far space, also called navigational space [14], extends beyond our reach and has been called the space within “walking distance” [15]. Our brain distinguishes between far and near space because the relevance of actions performed by others may be different for the observer according to the region of space in which they are executed and can lead to different behavioural responses. Recently, it was demonstrated that the dissociation between reaching and walking space might not be limited to perception and action but might extend to memory systems, as also shown by functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) [1619]. In particular, the calcarine cortex, lingual gyrus and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex on the right side of the brain were selectively involved in learning within navigational space, and the middle occipital gyrus, inferior temporal gyrus and lingual and fusiform gyrus on the left side of the brain were selectively involved in learning within reaching space [19]. Visuo-spatial memory allows us to remember where objects around us are and provides an overview map of space that can be used for navigation. Reaching and walking space can be distinguished in visuo-spatial memory. In fact, there is increasing evidence that remembering items within grasping distance is different from remembering a pathway within walking distance. Recent studies of brain-damaged patients suffering from navigational deficits showed that they selectively failed in topographical memory tasks but not in reaching memory tasks [1618]. Visuo-spatial memory is a multidimensional concept [2022]. Indeed, It is relevant in brain-damaged patients who show dissociated performances and in distinct patterns due to individual differences (i.e., gender, age, spatial styles and differences in expertise). In general, memory performance can be influenced by several external and internal factors and people often report perceived memory deficits that do not necessarily reflect objective changes [8, 23].

In this study we subjected a female Italian Air Force pilot (Case 1) to reaching and walking distance memory tests and a hormonal assessment in the second trimester of pregnancy and one year postpartum. Previously, we investigated the capability of Case 1, during pregnancy and one year postpartum, to mentally rotate an object finding the presence of deficits [24]. Mental rotation is a basic skill for a pilot and for solving visuo-spatial memory tests, it is always assessed during the selection for entering in the Air Force Academy and all pilots demonstrate high mental rotation abilities. This first investigation suggests us to scrutinize other cognitive processes related to mental rotation and visuo-spatial abilities. To this purpose, in the present paper we assessed the ability of Case 1 in reaching distance memory (this ability in Aviation is used in managing on-line information coming from the instrument panel) and walking distance memory (a memory for navigational information coming from the external environment), both this types of memory are basic in successful flying. To measure reaching distance memory we used the Corsi Block-Tapping test (CBT; [25]) and to assess walking distance memory, the Walking Corsi Test (WalCT; [16, 26]). The latter test is a validated navigational variant of the CBT, which requires observing and reproducing spatial sequences by walking in a room. We investigated Case 1’s performance with regard to temporal duration (working memory and long-term memory) in both the CBT and the WalCT.

During her second trimester of pregnancy she showed a large deficit in the CBT but not in the WalCT. She was aware of the deficit, which almost completely disappeared one year postpartum. By studying Case 1 we were able to gain more knowledge about the memory changes that occur during pregnancy also in women with high-spatial ability and no complaints of depression, sleep disruption or fatigue. The dissociation between her performance in the reaching and walking distance memory tests is noteworthy because her expertise should have mitigated her low walking space memory performance. This effect can be explained as a protective factor resulting from her experience in human navigation, which prevented her complete visuo-spatial memory failure by saving the topographical component.

Case presentation

Case 1 is a 32-year-old Italian female pilot of the Italian Air Force (ItAF). At the time of the study she had 680 hours of flight experience. She was assessed with reaching and walking distance memory tests twice in the second trimester of pregnancy and one year after delivery. Circulating levels of estradiol, progesterone and testosterone were measured on three different occasions during the second trimester of pregnancy and one year after delivery in the follicular phase of three different menstrual cycles. Estradiol levels were significantly different (p < .05), that is 798 ± 126 pMol/L and 181 ± 121 pMol/L. Progesterone was 151.6 ± 18.2 nMol/L during the second trimester and dropped to 1.9 ± 0.5 nMol/L (p < .05) during the follicular phase of the normal menstrual cycle. Finally, testosterone remained almost stable throughout the entire observation period: 2.2 ± 0.6 nMol/L during pregnancy vs. 2.9 ± 0.3 nMol/L postpartum (p = n.s.).

Her performance on the memory tests was compared with the performances of 10 women ItAF pilots (mean age 28.9 ± 2.8 years; mean education 18 ± 0) and with the validated normative data reported for the WalCT and CBT in Piccardi et al. [26]. Both groups (pilots and normative sample) were matched with Case 1 for age, gender and education.

The study was approved by the local ethical committee of the Experimental Flight Center, Aerospace Medicine Department, “M. de Bernardi ” Air Base (prot. n. 2012/09/24 RMAS), Italy. Written informed consent to participate in to the study was provided by Case 1 and by the control groups (both pilots and normative sample).

Reaching distance memory

The CBT [25] is a widely used visuo-spatial memory task in which nine blocks (4.5 × 4.5 cm) are fixed on a baseboard (30 × 25 cm) in a scattered array. It tests both working memory and long-term memory. To test working memory (WM), the examiner taps a number of blocks at a rate of one block per 2 s, after which the subject has to tap the block sequence in the same order. The block sequences gradually increase in length (starting from a 2-block sequence); the score is the number of blocks in the longest sequence remembered correctly (block span).

We assessed two aspects of visuo-spatial long-term memory: learning (L) and delayed recall (DR). In the L part of the test, she had to learn an eight-block sequence (following the procedure described in [16, 26]) demonstrated by the examiner. The learning criterion was reached if she reproduced the correct sequence three times in a row (maximum number of trials: 18). The learning score was calculated by attributing one point for each block correctly tapped until the criterion was reached; then it was added to the score corresponding to correct performance of the remaining trials (up to the 18th; maximum score: 144). Five minutes later, the DR part of the test was administered. The examiner asked Case 1 to reproduce the previously learned eight-block sequence. The score was the number of blocks correctly reproduced (maximum score: 8). She was tested individually in a quiet room with artificial lighting. She sat facing the examiner on a height-adjustable office chair in front of the CBT baseboard. Case 1’s performance on WM and the L and DR tests of the CBT was severely impaired when compared with the performances of the other women pilots and the women in the normative group (see Results section).

Walking distance memory

To assess her ability to learn and remember spatial locations during navigation we used the Walking Corsi Test (WalCT: 16, 26). In the WalCT, she had to reproduce a walked sequence (previously demonstrated by the examiner) and to stop at different locations. The WalCT is a larger version of the CBT (3 × 2.5 m; scale 1:10 of the CBT), which is set up in an empty room. It consists of nine squares placed on a carpet in the same positions as in the standard CBT. The examiner shows the sequence by walking on the carpet and stopping on each square for 2 s. In this study, she had to repeat the exact sequence by walking and stopping on the squares included in the sequence. Also in the WalCT, she had to perform three different tasks: topographical working memory (TWM), in which a square span was obtained; topographical learning (TL), in which she had to learn an eight-square sequence following the same procedure and adopting the same learning criterion as in the CBT; and topographical delayed recall (TDR), in which Case 1 had to perform the eight-square sequence after five minutes had elapsed.

Results showed that Case 1’s TWM did not differ from that of controls (i.e., women pilots and nonpilots). Case 1’s performance on the TL and TDR after five minutes was comparable to that of controls (see Table 1 and Results section).

Table 1 Case 1’s and average controls’ (i.e., pilots vs. nonpilots) performances in reaching and walking distance memory tests

Results

The performance of Case 1 and controls (i.e., both women pilots and nonpilots) was compared using Crawford and Howell’s ([27]; CH) analysis with the computer program SINGLIMS.EXE. This analysis uses a modified t-test, described in Sokal and Rohlf [28], and is the most suitable one for estimating the abnormality of individual scores when the normative sample is small (i.e., less than 50 subjects).

We also compared Case 1’s performance on the CBT and the WalCT to determine whether there were any dissociations in her performance of the two tasks. This comparison was made using the computer program DISSOCS.EXE. [29]. It showed Case 1’s discrepancy from the control sample by estimating the percentage of the control population that exhibited a more extreme discrepancy than that of Case 1. We also analysed Case 1’s performance during pregnancy and postpartum to determine whether she was recovering and the magnitude of the recovery. To assess this aspect we used the C_CTC.EXE [30] program, which compared the scores of two of the two different moments, that is, during pregnancy and postpartum, as two separate events.

Results for each test are described below and shown in Table 1.

Crawford analysis (DISSOCS.EXE) showed a classical dissociation between Case 1’s CBT learning (z score = -4.56) and WalCT learning (z score = 1.25) scores. Compared with the performance of the women pilots’ group, Case 1’s performance showed a significant discrepancy (t = 4.44; p < .01). In fact, only 0.08% of the control group performed more discrepantly than Case 1. This incongruous performance was no longer present postpartum (CBT z score = -0.784; WalCT z score = 0.288; t = .858; p = .41 n.s.). The discrepancy was also present when Case 1’s performance was compared with that of the non-pilot women’s group (CBT z score = -3.622; WalCT z score = 1.262; t = 4.146 p < .001). When we compared her performance in the CBT during and one year after delivery, a significant difference emerged in Case 1’s ability to learn in reaching distance (t = -2.67; p < .01); however, there was no significant difference in her WalCT performance (t = .68; p = n.s.). In reaching space, she also failed in working memory (see Table 1), but this discrepancy did not reach significance when compared with the performance of the pilots’ group (CBT z score = -1.905; WalCT z score = .915; t = 2.03; p = 0.07 n.s.) and the non-pilots’ group (CBT z score = -0.338; WalCT z score = 1.713; t = 1.792; p = .08 n.s.).

Discussion

Case 1 is an expert military pilot who showed dissociated performance in reaching versus topographical memory during pregnancy. To our knowledge this is the first investigation of specific aspects of visuo-spatial memory in a pregnant woman. She was evaluated during the second trimester of pregnancy and one year after delivery, when her hormonal values (estradiol and progesterone) had returned to normal. To investigate working memory and long-term memory she was submitted to the CBT and the WalCT on two different occasions. We compared her results during and after pregnancy with those of a group of pilots and a normative group matched for age, gender and education. At the first assessment (during pregnancy) she failed significantly in all aspects of reaching memory (working memory, learning and delayed recall). Surprisingly, all aspects of her topographical memory were preserved. This classical dissociation could be a specific effect of pregnancy or a protective effect due to her navigational expertise. However, Case 1’s poor performance during pregnancy, above all in reaching working memory, raises the issue of whether it was the pregnancy itself that impaired her performance or whether her baseline performance was simply worse than that of the controls. Indeed, we do not have a baseline of her performance before pregnancy and since her performance after pregnancy is not still completely recovered, even if improved, this could leave open the question if she could be worse than other women pilots also before her pregnancy. In any case, it is important to stress that a very strict selection is made at the beginning of the career in aviation that excludes people with low spatial abilities (including visuo-spatial working memory) and also rules out most women candidates. Indeed, last year the following data were published regarding candidates who attempted to enter the Aviation Academy: out of all women admitted to the selection only 66.67% passed the visuo-spatial test battery and only 16.67% of the remaining sample passed the mathematics tests. On the contrary, out of all men admitted to the selection 86.60% passed the visuo-spatial test battery and 44.04% of the remaining sample passed the mathematics tests. For this reason, we support the hypothesis that her low performance in the reaching memory task was a specific effect of hormonal changes during pregnancy and not an undiagnosed pre-pregnancy condition. Also her improvement after pregnancy in all other memory aspects suggests that she is coming back to her previous baseline. Indeed, she did not differ anymore from control groups in the postpartum period. It is also important to highlight that postpartum recovery in the general population is described as a broad spectrum ranging from two days to up to two months or longer [4, 31, 32] and no studies has still estimated the specific range in which these changes happened.

Sex differences in spatial abilities are not consistently found across studies because spatial ability encompasses three major dimensions: visualisation, orientation, and space relations [33]. Large differences that favour men are found in some visuo-spatial tasks, such as mental rotation, and spatiotemporal tasks [34]. Kimura and Hampson [35] reported that woman’s work performance changes in accordance with oestrogen fluctuations during their periods. In particular, when this hormone increases, spatial skills are minimized and manual and talking skills are maximized. Since visuo-spatial memory is also a multicomponential function, some components could be influenced more than others due to different hormonal levels in pregnancy and postpartum. In this case we could not exclude that a specific effect of navigational expertise preserved all aspects of topographical memory.

Caution should be taken in drawing conclusions about the absence of effects on topographical memory in pregnancy. Indeed, Piccardi et al. [16] found that men showed a larger span on the WalCT than on the CBT, whereas women’s spans did not differ on the two tests. Men were also faster than women in learning the supra-span sequence and in general were better in performing the WalCT than the CBT [16]. Women were slower in learning the path from a map and needed more repetitions to learn it in the real environment, but once they had learned the path there were no sex differences in delayed recall [36]. According to the hypothesis of Coluccia and Iosue [37] gender differences can be explained by the visuo-spatial WM load. Therefore, a WM failure during the typical hormonal changes of pregnancy could be in line with the difficulty of a control group of non-pregnant women in solving active working memory tasks. Indeed, sex differences are often reported in the CBT (see [38, 39]) that favour men with respect to women. Verde et al. [40] found that sex differences in mental rotation tasks were not present in a pilot population but that these differences were strong in a non-pilot population. In this study women pilots performed the same task as men pilots and as men non-pilots. During pregnancy, she performed similarly to the non-pilot women and performed even worse than this group in delayed recall. Although we were unable speculate about whether the general population of pregnant women would show a performance decline on the CBT, we can hypothesise that visuo-spatial working memory in reaching distance could have been impaired by the hormonal changes that occur during pregnancy. She did not show any deficits in the WalCT, thus showing that her topographical working memory was significantly larger than that of the non-pilot women. However, her walking memory performance could have been preserved due to a protective effect of her expertise in navigation regardless of her state. It has been widely demonstrated, however, that women prefer a route strategy based on egocentric memory for navigating (e.g., [4143]). Recently, Nemmi et al. [19] performed an fMRI study comparing CBT and WalCT learning sequences and found that the retrosplenial cortex was involved in performance of the WalCT. These authors found that activation in the region in which the calcarine sulcus joins the parieto-occipital sulcus, which has been found active in several navigational tasks [44, 45]. Indeed, according to Byrne and Becker [46] this area might also transform egocentric representations into an allocentric frame and vice versa. This anatomical functioning evidence indicates why an egocentric strategy might be advantageous in performing the WalCT with respect to the CTB. In fact, in other studies women showed a dishomogeneous pattern in performing the CBT and the WalCT, which suggests that they use egocentric strategies in both types of memory.

Another interesting point is that in reaching distance the temporal storage of memory was recovered in a different way, showing that WM but not long-term memory is particularly sensitive to hormonal fluctuations. This evidence is in line with the current idea that most sex differences in solving spatial cognitive tasks are related to the active visuo-spatial working memory load.

The assessment of Case 1 (i.e. a woman with high spatial ability and without any complaints of depression, sleep disruption or fatigue) improved our knowledge of the memory changes that occur during pregnancy. Furthermore, the dissociation between her performance in reaching and walking distance memory tests is interesting because her expertise could have mitigated her low walking space memory performance. This can be explained as a factor protecting her experience in human navigation and preventing the complete failure of her visuo-spatial memory by saving the topographical component. Note that a military pilot is engaged in a range of visuo-spatial cognitive tasks that require different types of memory, which might be recovered in varying ways at different times postpartum. In the literature, postpartum recovery in the general population is described as a broad spectrum ranging from two days to up to two months or longer [4, 31, 32].

The above observations must be carefully considered when women pilots resume flight duties after delivery and suggest that a detailed neuropsychological assessment should be made to investigate different aspects of memory.

Conclusions

The present case points out the importance of assessing spatial abilities that might be affected by hormonal changes during pregnancy and postpartum also in experts to guarantee their health and to determine when they should return from maternity leave.

Consent

Written informed consent was obtained from the patient for publication of this Case Report. A copy of the written consent is available for review by the Editor-in-Chief of this journal.

References

  1. Poser CM, Kassirer MR, Peyser JM: Benign encephalopathy of pregnancy: preliminary clinical observations. Acta Neurol Scand. 1986, 73: 39-43.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  2. Brett M, Baxendale S: Motherhood and memory: a review. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2001, 26: 339-362.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  3. de Groot RHM, Hornstra G, Roozendaal N, Jolles J: Memory performance, but not information processing speed, may be reduced during early pregnancy. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2003, 25: 482-488.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. de Groot RHM, Vuurman EFPM, Hornstra G, Jolles J: Differences in cognitive performance during pregnancy and early motherhood. Psychol Med. 2006, 36: 1023-1032.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  5. Casey P, Huntsdale C, Angus G, Janes C: Memory in pregnancy II: implicit, incidental, explicit, semantic, short term, working, and prospective memory in primigravid, multigravid, and post- partum women. J Psychosom Obstet Gynaecol. 1999, 1999 (20): 158-164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Crawley RA, Dennison K, Carter C: Cognition in pregnancy and the first year post-partum. Psychol Psychother. 2003, 76: 69-84.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  7. McDowall J, Moriarty R: Implicit and explicit memory in pregnant women: an analysis of data-driven and conceptually driven processes. Q J Exp Psychol. 2000, 53A: 729-740.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Henry JD, Rendell PG: A review of the impact of pregnancy on memory function. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2007, 28: 793-803.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Casey P: A longitudinal study of cognitive performance during pregnancy and new motherhood. Arch Womens Ment Health. 2000, 3: 65-76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Janes C, Casey P, Huntsdale C, Angus G: Memory in pregnancy I: Subjective experiences and objective assessment of implicit, explicit, and working memory in primigravid and primiparous women. J Psychosom Obstet Gynaecol. 1999, 20: 80-87.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. Keenan PA, Yaldoo DT, Stress ME, Fuerst DR, Ginsburg KA: Explicit memory in pregnant women. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 1998, 179: 731-737.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  12. Sharp K, Brindle PM, Brown MW, Turner GM: Memory loss during pregnancy. Br J Obstet Gynaecol. 1993, 100: 209-215.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  13. Rizzolatti G, Berti A, Gallese V: Spatial neglect: neurophysiological bases, cortical circuits and theories. Handbook of neuropsychology. Edited by: Boller F, Grafman J, Rizzolatti G. 2000, Amsterdam: Elsevier, 503-537.

    Google Scholar 

  14. De Nigris A, Piccardi L, Bianchini F, Palermo L, Incoccia C, Guariglia C: Role of visuo-spatial working memory in path-integration disorders in neglect. Cortex. 2013, 49 (4): 920-930.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Halligan PW, Marshall JC: Left neglect for near but not far space in man. Nature. 1991, 350 (6318): 498-500.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  16. Piccardi L, Iaria G, Ricci M, Bianchini F, Zompanti L, Guariglia C: Walking in the Corsi test: which type of memory do you need?. Neurosci Lett. 2008, 432: 127-131.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  17. Piccardi L, Berthoz A, Baulac M, Denos M, Dupont S, Samson S, Guariglia C: Different spatial memory systems are involved in small- and large-scale environments: evidence from patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. Exp Brain Res. 2010, 206: 171-177.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  18. Piccardi L, Iaria G, Bianchini F, Zompanti L, Guariglia C: Dissociated deficits of visuo-spatial memory in near space and navigational space: evidence from brain-damaged patients and healthy older participants. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. 2011, 18 (3): 362-384.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  19. Nemmi F, Boccia M, Piccardi L, Galati G, Guariglia C: Segregation of neural circuits involved in spatial learning in reaching and navigational space. Neuropsychologia. 2013, 51: 1561-1570.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Kessels RPC, de Haan EHF, Kappelle LJ, Postma A: Varieties of human spatial memory: a meta-analysis on the effects of hippocampal lesions. Brain Res Rev. 2001, 35: 295-303.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  21. Postma A: Taxonomy of spatial memory processes. Cogn Process. 2000, 1 (ICSC 2000): 52-

    Google Scholar 

  22. Schacter DL, Nadel L: Varieties Of Spatial Memory: A Problem For Cognitive Neuroscience. Perspectives on cognitive neuroscience. Edited by: Lister RG, Weingartner HJ. 1991, Oxford: Oxford University Press, 165-185.

    Google Scholar 

  23. Cook S, Marsiske M: Subjective memory beliefs and cognitive performance in normal and mildly impaired older adults. Aging Ment Health. 2006, 10: 413-423.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Piccardi L, Verde P, Bianchini F, Morgagni F, Guariglia C, Strollo F: Mental rotation task in a pilot during and after pregnancy. Aviat Space Environ Med. 2013, 84: 1092-1094.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Corsi PM: Human memory and the medial temporal region of the brain. Diss Abstr Int. 1972, 34 (02): 891B-University micro-lms No. AA105-77717

    Google Scholar 

  26. Piccardi L, Bianchini F, Argento O, De Nigris A, Maialetti A, Palermo L, Guariglia C: The Walking Corsi Test (WalCT): standardization of the topographical memory test in an Italian population. Neurol Sci. 2013, 34 (6): 971-978.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  27. Crawford JR, Howell DC: Comparing an individual’s test score against norms derived from small samples. Clin Neuropsychol. 1998, 12: 482-486.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Sokal RR, Rohlf FJ: Biometry: The Principles And Practice Of Statistics In Biological Research. 1995, New York: Freeman

    Google Scholar 

  29. Crawford JR, Garthwaite PH: Testing for suspected impairments and dissociations in single-case studies in neuropsychology: evaluation of alternatives using Monte Carlo simulations and revised tests for dissociations. Neuropsychology. 2005, 19: 318-331.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Crawford JR, Garthwaite PH, Woods LT: Inferential methods for comparing two single cases. Cogn Neuropsychol. 2010, 27: 377-400.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Eidelman AI, Hoffman NW, Kaitz M: Cognitive deficits in women after childbirth. Obstet Gynecol. 1993, 81: 764-767.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  32. Condon JT, Derham D, Kneebone AC: Cognitive functioning during pregnancy: a controlled investigation using psychometric testing. Int J Prenatal Perinatal Studies. 1991, 60: 199-212.

    Google Scholar 

  33. Choi J, Silverman I: Sexual dismorphism in spatial behaviours: applications to route learning. Evol Cogn. 1996, 2: 165-171.

    Google Scholar 

  34. Dabbs JM, Chang EL, Strong RA, Milun R: Spatial ability, navigation strategy, and geographic knowledge among men and women. Evol Hum Behav. 1998, 19: 89-98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Kimura D, Hampson E: Cognitive pattern in men and women is influenced by fluctuations in sex hormones. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 1994, 3: 57-61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Piccardi L, Risetti M, Nori R, Tanzilli A, Bernardi L, Guariglia C: Perspective changing in primary and secondary learning: a gender difference study. Learn Individ Differ. 2011, 21: 114-118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Coluccia E, Iosue G: Gender differences in spatial orientation: a review. J Environ Psychol. 2004, 24: 329-340.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Orsini A, Schiappa O, Grossi D: Sex and cultural differences in children’s spatial and verbal memory span. Percept Mot Skills. 1981, 53: 39-42.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  39. Orsini A, Grossi D, Capitani E, Laiacona M, Papagno C, Vallar G: Verbal and spatial immediate memory span: normative data from 1355 adults and 1112 children. It J Neurol Sci. 1987, 8: 539-548.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  40. Verde P, Piccardi L, Bianchini F, Trivelloni P, Guariglia C, Tomao E: Gender effects on mental rotation in pilots vs. nonpilots. Aviat Space Environ Med. 2013, 84: 726-729.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Lawton CA: Gender differences in way-finding strategies: relationship to spatial ability and spatial anxiety. Sex Role. 1994, 30: 765-779.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Lawton CA: Strategies for indoor wayfinding: the role of orientation. J Environ Psychol. 1996, 16: 137-145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Saucier DM, Green SM, Leason J, MacFadden A, Bell S, Elias LJ: Are sex differences in navigation caused by sexually dimorphic strategies or by differences in the ability to use the strategies?. Behav Neurosci. 2002, 116: 403-410.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Iaria G, Chen JK, Guariglia C, Ptito A, Petrides M: Retrosplenial and hippocampal brain regions in human navigation: complementary functional contributions to the formation and use of cognitive maps. Eur J Neurosci. 2007, 25 (3): 890-899.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Maguire EA, Burgess N, Donnett JG, Frackowiak RSJ: Knowing where and getting there: a human navigation network. Science. 1998, 280 (5365): 921-924.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  46. Byrne P, Becker S: Remembering the past and imaging the future a neural model of spatial memory and imagery. Psychol Rev. 2007, 114 (2): 340-375.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Authors are grateful to Case 1 for her kind collaboration. The authors declare they have no financial support to declare.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Paola Verde.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

LP was chief investigator of the study and was responsible for the first and last draft of the manuscript. PV carried out the literature search, study design, data interpretation and writing. FB, FM and CG were principal investigators of a study site, contributed to recruitment and research monitoring and worked on data collection. LP and CG carried out the data analysis, interpreted the results and wrote the manuscript. FS was the principal investigator concerned with the hormonal analysis; he read, interpreted and wrote the hormonal results. ET was head of the medical corps, authorized the study, participated in the study design and in all phases of the work. All authors participated in data acquisition and revision and critical review of the manuscript, and all have seen and approved the final version.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.

Authors’ original file for figure 1

Rights and permissions

Open Access  This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made.

The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder.

To view a copy of this licence, visit https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (https://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Piccardi, L., Verde, P., Bianchini, F. et al. Deficits in visuo-spatial but not in topographical memory during pregnancy and the postpartum state in an expert military pilot: a case report. BMC Res Notes 7, 524 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1186/1756-0500-7-524

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1756-0500-7-524

Keywords