Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Painfully thin but locked inside a fatter body: abnormalities in both anticipation and execution of action in anorexia nervosa

Abstract

Background

People with anorexia nervosa (AN) usually report feeling broader than they really are. The objective of the present study was to better understand the body schema's involvement in this false self-representation in AN. We tested the potential for correction of the body schema impairment via the sensorimotor feedback provided by a real, executed action and relative to an imagined action. We also took account of the impact of the AN patients’ weight variations on the task outcomes.

Methods

Fourteen inpatient participants with AN and fourteen control participants were presented with a doorway-like aperture. The participants had to (i) judge whether or not various apertures were wide enough for them to pass through in a motor imagery task and then (ii) actually perform the action by passing through various apertures.

Results

We observed a higher passability ratio (i.e. the ratio between the critical aperture size and shoulder width) in participants with AN (relative to controls) for both motor imagery and real action. Moreover, the magnitude of the passability ratio was positively correlated with weight recovery.

Conclusion

The body schema alteration in AN appears to be strong enough to affect the patient's actions. Furthermore, the alteration resists correction by the sensorimotor feedback generated during action. This bias is linked to weight variations. The central nervous system might be locked to a false representation of the body that cannot be updated. Moreover, these results prompt us to suggest that emotional burden during weight recovery could also alter sensorimotor aspects of body representation. New therapeutic methods should take account of body schema alterations in AN as adjuncts to psychotherapy.

Background

"Being so many different sizes in a day is very confusing". Just as with Alice in Wonderland, body mass changes may not only affect a person's feelings about his/her body but may also affect the sensorimotor system. Both before and after massive, rapid weight loss (as in anorexia nervosa (AN), a serious mental illness that predominantly affects young women[1]), patients often consider that they are fatter and broader than they really are[2]. This "disturbance in the way in which one's body weight or shape is experienced" is a major clinical symptom of AN[3] but is also a risk factor that drives the obsessive will to lose weight and contributes to the maintenance of restrictive eating behaviors[4]. Thus, there is an urgent need to gain a better understanding of the process underlying body overestimation in AN, with a view to developing novel remediation techniques as adjuncts to psychotherapy.

From a psychopathological point of view, factors such as attachment style (insecurity of attachment and the need for approval) and depressive symptoms (negative judgments about oneself, worshipping others) have a crucial role in body dissatisfaction[5] and are probably also linked to body overestimation[6]. From a neuropsychological point of view, body overestimation in AN provides clues as to how the body representation is built and then adjusted as a function of weight changes and related to body width changes[7]. In this context, researchers draw a distinction between two major body representations (for a review, see[8]): the body image and the body schema. Body image is considered to be a conscious representation of the body in terms of its esthetical, emotional and social components. In contrast, body schema is viewed as a sensorimotor representation of the body that is involved whenever a motor action is imagined or performed[811]. Many of the studies to date have stressed the disruption of the conscious body image in AN[12]. Only a few authors have suggested that the involvement of a body schema distortion[7, 1317] is related to the putative dysfunction of the parietal cortex (and the right parietal lobule in particular) in AN[18, 19], since that this cortical area was found to be crucial for emergence of the neural network that establishes a coherent body schema[20].

Considering that body schema is involved in the overall process of action (from the anticipation through to execution) and in view of literature evidence that simulation of action by motor imagery and motor execution share kinematic and neural properties[2123], our group first looked at possible repercussions of this abnormal body schema in AN by using a first-person visuomotor imagery task[15, 16]. In fact, we applied an aperture passability task similar to that used in Experiment 2 of a seminal study[24]. This paradigm has been further used to investigate emotional/social influences on spatial perception[25] and to evidence motor dysfunctions in Parkinson's disease[26, 27]. In our previous experiments[15, 16], AN patients and control participants had to judge (without executing the action) whether or not an aperture was wide enough for them to pass through. Hence, the patients judged that they could not pass through an aperture without turning their shoulders, even when it was easily wide enough for them. In fact the patients responded as if their body was larger than it really was - confirming the clinical complaint of feeling fat that has been evaluated with various body image questionnaires[2830] and contour drawing scales[31, 32].

Although these motor imagery abnormalities were relevant for highlighting the involvement of the body schema, the importance of testing the impact of these abnormalities on real actions was suggested by Guardia et al.[16] and tested in a case study[7]. Indeed, Keizer et al.[17] recently investigated whether disturbed experience of body image also extended to body schema in a body-scaled action in AN. To better understand body schema changes as a core symptom of AN, we decided to further investigate the role of action in this disorder by comparing imagined and real action in a same experiment. Indeed, humans demonstrate a remarkable ability to generate accurate, appropriate motor behavior under the changing environmental conditions that they encounter in daily life. It has been suggested that the central nervous system uses a modular approach in which multiple controllers co-exist and are selected according to the context of the movement (such as the size of surrounding objects that could be obstacles[33]). According to this hypothesis and in line with Frith’s model[34], the desired action (passing through an aperture, for instance) creates a motor sequence that can be adjusted during the realization of action by the motor control system as a function of the environmental constraints (the size of an aperture, for instance). People with AN may misperceived the critical aperture when simulating the action (i.e. in a motor imagery task) because their internal model is based on an incorrect body schema. However, if they pass through many different apertures in a given session, AN patients might be able to correct their pattern of movements and compensate for their impaired body schema in a "learning-by doing" process[35]. Moreover, testing the execution of an action is an implicit means of evoking the body schema by preventing top-down processes (such as the intrusion of an impaired conscious body image). This approach is especially pertinent in AN, in view of the recent suggestion[36] that patients are locked to a predominant, allocentric body image related to their previous body size and weight (possibly due to rapid and massive weight loss). Hence, this allocentric body image might alter the updating of their new body schema.

Thus, in the present study, we sought to test the hypothesis whereby AN patients not only think they are fatter but behave as if they were fatter in an activity of daily life because they are unable to correct their internal model via external and proprioceptive inputs provided by execution of the action. Hence, we sought to establish whether anorexic patients not only misperceive the critical aperture but also behave wrongly accordingly. To this end, we recorded the actual gait and movements of AN patients as they approached and passed through different apertures (i.e. the real action). A study of a real action task[24] showed that there is a critical aperture width through which the healthy participant could no longer pass without turning sideways to some extent (a πi ratio of 1.3). This value means that the participant first turns sideways when the aperture is 1.3 times wider than the participant's shoulder width. Moreover, this rotation is initiated before the passage and is body-scaled. These actions were also compared with the results of the mental imagery task. Moreover, our method is quantitative and enables a statistical analysis of the clinician's observations concerning body overestimation and dissatisfaction. Our starting hypothesis was that if body schema is affected in AN, patients not only overestimate their critical aperture more than control participants do but also behave as if their bodies were wider by turning their shoulders for a wider critical aperture than control participants do. In both imagery and action, the passability ratio should be higher in the AN group than in the control group.

Methods

Ethics statement

This study was approved by a regional independent ethics committee (Comité de Protection des Personnes Nord-Ouest IV; study number: 2007-A01413-50). The study complied with the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki. Each participant received a study information sheet and provided her written, informed consent to participation. Parental consent was additionally required for participants under the age of 18.

Participants

The demographic and clinical characteristics of the study population are summarized in Table 1. The study included 28 young female participants: 14 AN inpatients recruited at the hospital and 14 healthy, controls (matched for educational level and age), recruited from the hospital's medical students and medical staff. Neither AN patients nor controls had participated in our previous experiments. Although the study participants had been informed about the study's general objective and procedures, they were not told about the starting hypotheses or the expected results. All 14 patients fulfilled the DSM IV-TR criteria for AN, with 12 restricting types[37]. Administration of the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview[38] by a psychiatrist confirmed the absence of other psychiatric pathologies (according to the DSM IV criteria) in the two groups. All the control participants had a normal BMI (i.e. weight/height2 ranging from 18.5 to 25 kg/m2). Male AN sufferers were not recruited, given their low prevalence and the high rate of psychiatric comorbidities in this population. A neuropsychiatric interview of the participants did not reveal any perceptual, attentional or intellectual impairments. The participants were asked to mention all ongoing courses of medication or other treatments (such as physiotherapy, osteopathy, and psychomotricity) and any previous hospitalizations due to sensorimotor problems. People with a history of neurological, ophthalmic or sensorimotor problems or with ongoing or previous treatments with strongly psychotropic medications were excluded. The experiment lasted approximately one and a half hours.

Table 1 Demographic and clinical data for the anorexia nervosa (AN) and control groups

Materials and procedures

Morphological and clinical parameters

An investigator measured each participant's height and shoulder width. The "bi-acromial" shoulder width was measured with a medical measuring rod with 1 mm graduations, and corresponded to the distance between the outside edges of the shoulders' acromion processes. In view of the patients' emotional burden, bodyweight was measured twice a week by hospital staff (as part of the patient's normal care) and not by the investigators. The last body weight indicated in the patient's medical records was considered to be the current value. Changes over time in nutritional states were measured by considering the patient's current body weight and the values measured by medical staff one month and six months before the study (which were also available in the patients’ medical records). Only the weight just before the beginning of the dieting and weight loss period (i.e. just prior to the onset of AN) was self-reported. Body dissatisfaction and concerns about weight and shape were assessed in both control and AN groups by administering the Body Shape Questionnaire (BSQ) and the Eating Disorder Inventory-2 (EDI-2), respectively. On the day of the study, these questionnaires were administered at the end of the experiment (to avoid emotional arousal and perturbation during experimental tasks).

The BSQ[29] is an one-dimensional, 34-item self-questionnaire that assesses concerns about body shape over the preceding 4 weeks. Answers are scored on a 6-point Likert scale of 1 (not present) to 6 (always present). The EDI-2[30] is a self-questionnaire with 11 subscales measuring psychological features commonly associated with eating disorders. Ninety-one items are rated on a Likert scale of 1 (never) to 6 (always).

Evaluation of passability

The "doorway-like" apertures were formed by two vertical poles (height: 1.80 m). The participant first stood 5 m away the aperture. The width between the two poles could be adjusted to between 36 cm to 80 cm in stepwise, 2 cm increments (in increasing and decreasing series). Two experimental conditions were tested.

In the motor imagery condition, the participants were instructed to imagine (without moving from their starting position) whether or not they would be able to walk through the presented apertures. The participants were told to answer " yes" if they thought it would be possible for them to pass through the aperture at a normal walking speed (as if they were at home, walking from one room to another) without having to turn sideways or to change their posture. If they thought that this was not the case, the participants were told to answer "no". The imagery was repeated twice with increasingly narrow apertures (yielding two descending series) and twice with increasingly wide apertures (yielding two ascending series). A descending series was stopped when there was a transition from a "yes" reply to two consecutive "no" replies. An ascending series was stopped when there was a transition from a "no" reply to two consecutive "yes" replies.

In the action condition, participants were instructed to walk through the aperture at a comfortable pace and to turn sideways if they so wished (in line with[24]). Next, they were asked to go as far as the wall behind the aperture, stop for 5 seconds, turn around and returned to the starting position by passing through the aperture a second time. In both groups, action ratios were always greater than 1 (the lowest was 1.04), meaning that none of the participants touched the poles. The condition was performed once with increasingly narrow apertures (yielding a descending series) and once with increasingly wide apertures (yielding an ascending series). The participant was filmed with two cameras (50 Hz) - one on the left side of the room in which the experiment was performed, and the other on the right side. These provided objective measures of shoulder rotation for each passage through the aperture. The starting point (5 m from the aperture) was the same for all participants and was marked on the ground. It was parallel to the aperture and centered relative to the left and right poles, so that the participant's approach was perpendicular to the aperture. For each aperture, we looked for shoulder rotation from about 2 m from the aperture; 2 m is the distance in which abnormally high shoulder rotation (i.e. greater than natural body sway) usually appears when approaching a critical aperture or a narrower aperture[39]. The room's lay-out is shown diagrammatically in Figure 1.

Figure 1
figure 1

The experimental set-up during the action task.

The motor imagery condition was always performed before the action condition. The action condition could not be performed first, since it would have improved performance in the motor imagery condition. In both conditions, participants had to close their eyes between trials so that they could not (i) see the poles being adjusted or (ii) using the experimenter’s body as a benchmark for judging the width of the aperture. Participants did not receive feedback on their performance.

Measurements of critical apertures and ratios

For both motor imagery and action conditions, we calculated a passability threshold (i.e. the critical aperture) and a ratio.

The motor imagery threshold (i.e. the critical aperture in motor imagery condition) in each series was defined as the average width at which the participants thought it was no longer possible to pass through without turning the shoulders. For each series, the critical aperture was the mean of the two successive aperture widths for which a change in reply ("yes, I could pass through the aperture without turning my shoulders" or "no, I could not") was obtained. We then computed the mean motor imagery critical aperture over the four motor imagery series. Lastly, a motor imagery ratio (πi) was computed by dividing the average motor imagery critical aperture by the participant’s shoulder width.

The motor threshold (i.e. the critical aperture in the action condition) was the mean width at which the subject began to turn her shoulders to an extent that exceeded a baseline level of natural rotational body sway[39]. The two cameras used the same time code and were synchronized electronically via a control box (model DT9817, Data Translation, Marlboro, MA, USA). Video data were processed with three-dimensional motion capture and analysis software (Simi Motion®, Simi Reality Motion Systems GmbH, Unterschleissheim, Germany). We used a cubic, calibrated space measuring 90 cm in length (the y axis), 80 cm in width (the x axis) and 180 cm in height (the z axis) (see Figure 2). The cameras were calibrated before each participant's test session. The shoulder rotation angles were determined from a spherical light sensor positioned on each of the participant's shoulders. These angles were compared with the aperture's plane (i.e. the vertical plane defined by the x and z axes; see Figure 2).

Figure 2
figure 2

Diagram of the calibration space and the vertical plane used for angle measurement (as viewed by the participant).

The measurement accuracy for the shoulder movements was 0.5 cm. The angle was extracted for each captured image (yielding 25 angles per second, given that the cameras’ frame rate was 50 Hz). Shoulder rotation angles could be either positive or negative, due to the body's natural left and right rotational sway during locomotion. Given that we wanted to focus on the emergence of shoulder rotation, we were interested in absolute values and their variation. Hence, we tried to determine the magnitude of shoulder rotation as accurately as possible if it occurred. For that, we chose to consider deviations above a predefined baseline rotation, as per[39]. For each aperture width, we then calculated a shoulder rotation index (referred to as henceforth as the R-index) as follows: (maximum angle in the dataset) - (mean deviation). The maximum angle was the largest absolute angle (in degrees) recorded for a given passage and aperture width. The mean deviation was the mean deviation between the absolute values of the various angles measured during gait and the mean of these angles. After having computed the R-indexes for each ascending or descending set, the critical aperture was defined graphically (as per[24]) as the first aperture after the inflexion point in the curve (see Figure 3).

Figure 3
figure 3

The R-index (the maximum rotation angle minus the mean deviation of all the rotation angles) as a function of aperture width.

Two laboratory members (acting as independent observers) were blinded to the participant's status (AN or control) and determined the critical aperture, as defined above. The correlation coefficient r for inter-rater agreement was 0.98. We then computed the mean motor critical aperture over the two series. Lastly, an action ratio (πa) was computed by dividing the motor critical aperture by the participant’s shoulder width.

Statistical analysis

All analyses were performed with Statistica software (version 7.1, Statsoft Inc., Tulsa, OK). When the conditions for application of a parametric test were fulfilled (i.e. a normal data distributions and homogeneity of variances), we applied Student's T-test or an analysis of variance (ANOVA) when comparing means and the Bravais-Pearson test when calculating correlations. Non-parametric Mann–Whitney and Spearman tests were used when non-normal distributions and/or non-homogenous inter-group variances were observed.

Results

Morphological and clinical parameters

Our analysis of the participants' morphological and clinical parameters is summarized in Table 1. There were no significant inter-group differences in terms of age (MeanAN ± SD =24.14 ± 9.65 years vs. MeanC ± SD = 25.21 ± 7.77 years, t26 = 0.32, p =0.74), or height (MeanAN = 1.66 ± 0.05 cm vs. MeanC = 1.67 ± 0.08 cm, t26 = 0.24, p = 0.85). In contrast, the mean current BMI was significantly lower for AN patients (MeanAN = 14.70 ± 1.50) than for controls (MeanC = 21.62 ± 1.82, t26 = 10.98, p <0.0001). Median shoulder width was also significantly lower in the AN group (MedAN = 35.7 cm) than in the control group (MedC = 39.75 cm, U =13.50, Z =3.88, p =0.001), reflecting the patients' state of malnutrition.

The mean time since disease onset was 5.71 ± 9.67 years. When compared with the control participants, the AN group had a significantly higher EDI-2 total score (MedAN = 112.5, MedC = 34.5; U =10, Z = -4.04, p <0.0001) and subscale scores ("drive for thinness"; MedAN = 14.5; MedC = 2, U =32.50, Z = -3.01, p = 0.002; "body dissatisfaction"; MedAN = 19, MedC = 6.5, U =26, Z = -3.30, p = <0.001). The BSQ scores were also significantly greater in the AN group than in the control group (MedAN = 137.5, MedC = 80.5, U =37.50, Z = -2.78, p =0.005). In order to analyze the effect of weight changes, two measurements were taken into account: the lowest BMI reached during the course of the disease (MeanAN =13.26 ± 1.30) and the difference between the present BMI and the lowest BMI (MeanAN =1.28 ± 1.10).

Evaluation of passability

The results for the motor imagery and action tasks are summarized in Table 2.

Table 2 Critical aperture, imagery and action ratios in the anorexia nervosa (AN) group and in the healthy control group

In the imagery condition, the individual πi ratios were much higher in most patients than in controls, with median ratios of 1.27 and 1.09 for the respective groups (U = 26, Z = -3.30, p < 0.001). Similar differences were observed in the action task, with higher πa ratios in most patients than in controls. The median ratio was 1.34 in the AN group and 1.16 in the control group. The significant difference in the action ratio (πa) was confirmed by statistical analysis (U =19, Z = -3.63, p <0.001).

In order to establish whether the AN group's ratio could be improved by repeating the action, we performed a 2×2 analysis of variance with repeated measures on the series factor and with group as a categorical predictor for the action ratio. This ANOVA revealed a significant effect of group (F(1,26) = 27.76, p < 0.0001). The AN group had significant higher action ratio (Mean = 1.32) than the control group (Mean = 1.16). Moreover, the effect of series was not statistically significant (F(1,26) = 3.80, p = 0.06). The first series yielded a lower action ratio (Mean = 1.23) than the second series (Mean = 1.26). The interaction between series and group was not statistically significant: F(1,26) = 1.40, p = 0.25 (see Figure 4). However, an a priori comparison revealed that the lower action ratio in the AN group was significantly lower in the first series (Mean = 1.29) than in the second series (Mean = 1.35). In contrast, no such difference was observed for the control group when comparing the first series (Mean =1.16) with the second series (Mean = 1.18; F(1,26) < 1).

Figure 4
figure 4

The mean action ratio as a function of the series (i.e. the first series vs. the second series) and the group. AN = patients suffered from anorexia nervosa. The vertical bars represent the 95% confidence interval.

Calculation of Spearman’s correlation coefficient (ρ) revealed a positive relationship between the motor imagery ratio and the action ratio for the study population as a whole (n = 28; ρ = 0.55; p < 0.05; one-tailed test). Our analysis did not reveal a significant correlation between the participant's height and the passability ratios for either the motor imagery condition (ρ = -0.21; p > 0.20) or the action condition (ρ = -0.17; p > 0.20).

In the AN group, the imagery ratio (πi) was not related to the BMI before the disease (ρ = 0.06; p >0.50), the BMI one month before the study (ρ = -0.01; p > 0.50) or the BMI weight six months before the study (ρ = -0.21; p > 0.20). However, πi was related to the difference between the BMI at the date of the experiment less the lowest BMI reached during the disease (weight recovery; ρ = 0.55, p <0.05 in a one-tailed test). Likewise, there were no significant correlations between these variables and the action ratio (πa) (all p > 0.10).

Discussion

In terms of perception of one's own body, patients suffering from AN have a very high level of body dissatisfaction. This reflects many different psychological factors. Attachment style and depressive symptoms were found to be more predictive of body dissatisfaction than personality traits were[5]. Body overestimation (i.e. a persistent feeling of being fatter than in reality) is linked to body dissatisfaction[6] and is a very complex symptom. It reflects many different false representations of the body (i.e. body schema as well as body image). Conscious, emotional and esthetic body overestimation may be embedded in the body schema at the sensorimotor level. To investigate the body schema's role in AN, we used ecological tasks in which the participant had to judge her likely ability to pass through apertures of differing widths (in the motor imagery task) or to actually pass through them (in the action task). The action and motor imagery tasks used in the present study enabled us to quantify and statistically analyze what the clinician usually has to assess with "yes-no" questionnaire answers during a consultation.

Overall, our results showed that AN patients not only overestimated the critical aperture in the motor imagery task but also turned sideways for a wider aperture than controls when they had to really pass through the aperture. Firstly, the present overestimation in the motor imagery task confirms our previous results[15, 16] but was evidenced in a more ecological paradigm; here, patients were faced with a physical aperture rather than the projected (two-dimensional) doorway-like aperture used in our previous experiments.

Secondly, the similar passability overestimation for imagery and a real action task means that anorexic patients not only believe that they cannot pass through an aperture that is nevertheless wide enough for them but also behave as if this was the case. When the action was really performed, our results confirm the findings reported recently by Keizer et al.[17]. In the present experiment, both motor imagery and action impairments observed in the AN group provide evidence in favor of an overestimated body schema in this disorder. Moreover, the ratio did not change significantly during the real executed action (action task) and no motor optimization between the two series was evidenced. This result is not worthy for several reasons. In the action task, the patients might have been able to correct their motor imagery "error" when walking towards or through the aperture via integration of environmental cues[33, 35] into the motor program. A motor imagery "error" would consist of a participant turning her shoulders for an aperture that was easily wide enough to pass through without a postural change. After having performing a number of actual passages, the participant might also have been able to correct her "errors" by integrating feedback ("reality check") from previous trials[33]. In contrast, our present results suggest that the body schema alteration in AN patients was strong enough to resist correction by external cues and performance of the action.

Although our results argue strongly in favor of a body schema alteration in AN, the origin of this alteration is another question. In order to assess possible links between body schema disruption and the weight variation, we took account of the patient's weight before the onset of AN (as reported by the patients themselves), the weight one month and six months before the experiment and the weight at the date of the experiment. It must be noted that apart from the weight before the disease onset, the measurement during the course of the disease were made during normal medical care (i.e. during the weight recovery process). According to the "allocentric lock" hypothesis[36], anorexic patients could be trapped in an outdated body schema – perhaps because of rapid, massive weight loss. However, we did not have any information about body schema evaluation prior to the onset of AN. It is likely that the patient's body schema was already impaired. Moreover, the absence of a significant positive correlation between bodyweight before AN and either of the two passability ratios argues against the "allocentric lock" hypothesis. Nevertheless, the presence of certain study limitations means that we cannot totally reject this hypothesis. In particular, the strong emotional burden attached to the self-reported "weight before the disease" might be a major source of bias. This is already indeed the case in the general population[40]. Even though AN patients self-reported their weight very precisely (to within a gram, according to our data; Table 2), they may have overestimated this weight (probably unconsciously) in order to reduce the amount of weight they had to recover and/or because they had considered themselves to be too fat before the onset of AN.

By contrast, we found a significant, positive correlation between weight recovery (i.e. the current BMI less the minimum BMI) and the motor imagery ratio; the greater the weight recovery during the preceding six months, the higher (and thus more biased) the motor imagery ratio. This unexpected result prompted one of the present article's reviewers to suggest a new hypothesis. In anorexic patients, weight regain (with calorie fear and false beliefs about being "obese") is associated with limbic and paralimbic activation and has consequences on emotional arousal (i.e. the presence of obsessive and depressive symptoms)[41]. During the weight recovery phase, the AN inpatient group's depression and anxiety scores were pathological. One could imagine that this emotional arousal interferes with visual and somesthetic sensory inputs that underpin body perception and motor expression. This hypothesis should be testing in future research (particularly in neuro-imaging studies).

Our analysis did not enable us to identify the origin of the distorted body schema. Hence, links between weight variation and body schema distortions should be investigated further. For instance, longitudinal studies over 6, 12 or 18 months would probably provide useful information on this question. However, in view of the rapid weight loss and/or regain in AN, only a longitudinal study running from the pre-puberty period (before the onset of AN) to adulthood (after AN) could clearly identify whether the distortion in the body schema was longstanding or recently acquired.

Although the above results again confirmed the role of body schema in both action and motor imagery tasks in AN, the present study had several limitations. Firstly, the small sample size and the heterogeneity of the patient group (in terms of ethnic and geographic factors, and prior hospitalizations and treatments) may have reduced the power of our analysis. Secondly, we were not able to test influence of the duration of AN, since its range was too limited (only two participants had suffered from AN for more than 10 years). Thirdly, for technical reasons, the apertures were not randomly presented. This is especially important for patients with AN, since the latter display less cognitive flexibility than controls[42, 43]. Although we cannot completely rule out an effect of non-random presentation on performance in the motor action task, it is noteworthy that our present results for the motor imagery task, are very similar to those found previously with random sequences[15, 16]. Lastly, cautious behavior in the AN group (turning the shoulders or not expecting to pass through the aperture) might mirror the AN patients’ obsession with perfectionism[44, 45]. However, we checked for correlations between perfectionist traits (evaluated on the EDI-2's "perfectionism" subscale) and the two ratios of passability in the AN group. None achieved statistical significance (motor imagery ratio: ρ = -0.32, p > .20; action ratio: ρ = -0.26, p > .20). Even the non-significant trends in these correlations were negative: the higher the perfectionism score, the lower the ratios. Hence, it is difficult to infer that the higher ratios in the AN group were entirely due to perfectionist traits.

Conclusions

In conclusion, a false feeling of being fat (a key symptom of AN) is not only confined to a conscious image of oneself but might affected the basic, sensorimotor representation of the body. This sensorimotor distortion might be modulated by the amount of weight recovery. The overall results of our experiment, even if they do not afford a new theoretical idea, reinforce the assumption of a body schema alteration in this psychiatric pathology. Our present findings may also open up new scientific perspectives. Future longitudinal studies will enable us to better understand (i) the origin of body schema distortion and (ii) the possible link between body schema distortion and the frequency of relapse (i.e. loss of weight after hospitalization, and maintenance of restraining behaviors).

Given that our present findings emphasize the key role of body schema in this psychiatric disorder, they also indicate the likely importance of evaluating and modulating the body schema in AN. In this perspective, therapies based on direct physical body experiences (physiotherapy, massage therapy, etc.) might be particularly valuable for countering body size overestimation in AN[46, 47]. In the same way, the use of virtual reality methods (in which body representation can be extensively manipulated[48]) represent key therapeutic perspectives for body schema rehabilitation in AN. We suggest that these virtual reality therapies should be based on body-scaled actions. By consciously confronting the patients with what they think they can (or cannot) do and what they can actually do (i.e. being able to pass through an appropriately wide aperture without turning their shoulders, for instance), it may be possible to improve their body representation. Indeed, the benefits of combining virtual reality treatment with cognitive behavioral therapies has been proven, with maintenance of these benefits after one year of follow-up[49].

Abbreviations

AN:

Anorexia Nervosa

BMI:

Body Mass Index

DSM:

Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders

BSQ:

Body Shape Questionnaire

EDI:

Eating Disorder Inventory.

References

  1. Hoek HW, Van Hoeken D: Review of the prevalence and incidence of eating disorders. Int J Eat Disord. 2003, 34: 383-396. 10.1002/eat.10222.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Cooper MJ, Deepak K, Grocutt E, Bailey E: The experience of " Feeling Fat " in women with anorexia nervosa, dieting and non-dieting women: an exploratory study. Eur Eat Disord Rev. 2007, 372 (February): 366-372.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 2013, Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing, 5

    Google Scholar 

  4. Heilbrun A, Friedberg L: Distorted body image in normal college women: possible implications for the development of anorexia nervosa. J Clin Psychol. 1990, 46: 398-401. 10.1002/1097-4679(199007)46:4<398::AID-JCLP2270460404>3.0.CO;2-5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Abbate-Daga G, Gramaglia C, Amianto F, Marzola E, Fassino S: Attachment insecurity, personality, and body dissatisfaction in eating disorders. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2010, 198: 520-524. 10.1097/NMD.0b013e3181e4c6f7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Fairburn C: Cognitive Behavior Therapy and Eating Disorders. 2008, New York: Guilford Press

    Google Scholar 

  7. Guardia D, Metral M, Pigeyre M, Bauwens I, Cottencin O, Luyat M: Body distortions after massive weight loss: lack of updating of the body schema hypothesis. Eat Weight Disord Stud Anorexia Bulim Obes. 2013, 18: 333-336. 10.1007/s40519-013-0032-0.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. De Vignemont F: Body schema and body image–pros and cons. Neuropsychologia. 2010, 48: 669-680. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.09.022.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Paillard J: Body schema and body image - a double dissociation in deafferented patients. Motor Control Today Tomorrow. Edited by: Gantchev G, Mori S, Massion J. 1999, Marseille: Academic Publishing House "Prof. M. Drinov", 198-213.

    Google Scholar 

  10. Gallagher S: Body image and body schema: a conceptual clarification. J Mind Behav. 1986, 7: 541-554.

    Google Scholar 

  11. Schwoebel J, Coslett HB: Evidence for multiple, distinct representations of the human body. J Cogn Neurosci. 2005, 17: 543-553. 10.1162/0898929053467587.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Cash TE, Deagle EA: The nature and the extent of body-image disturbances in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa: a meta-analysis. Int J Eat Disord. 1997, 22: 107-125. 10.1002/(SICI)1098-108X(199709)22:2<107::AID-EAT1>3.0.CO;2-J.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  13. Guardia D, Carey A, Cottencin O, Thomas P, Luyat M: Disruption of spatial task performance in anorexia nervosa. PLoS One. 2013, 8: e54928-10.1371/journal.pone.0054928.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Guardia D, Cottencin O, Thomas P, Dodin V, Luyat M: Spatial orientation constancy is impaired in anorexia nervosa. Psychiatry Res. 2012, 195: 56-59. 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.08.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Guardia D, Conversy L, Jardri R, Lafargue G, Thomas P, Dodin V, Cottencin O, Luyat M: Imagining one’s own and someone else's body actions: dissociation in anorexia nervosa. PLoS One. 2012, 7: e43241-10.1371/journal.pone.0043241.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Guardia D, Lafargue G, Thomas P, Dodin V, Cottencin O, Luyat M: Anticipation of body-scaled action is modified in anorexia nervosa. Neuropsychologia. 2010, 48: 3961-3966. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.09.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Keizer A, Smeets MAM, Dijkerman HC, Uzunbajakau SA, Van Elburg A, Postma A: Too fat to fit through the door: first evidence for disturbed body-scaled action in anorexia nervosa during locomotion. PLoS One. 2013, 8: e64602-10.1371/journal.pone.0064602.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Grunwald M, Ettrich C, Busse F, Assmann B, Dähne A, Gertz H-J: Angle paradigm: a new method to measure right parietal dysfunctions in anorexia nervosa. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2002, 17: 485-496. 10.1093/arclin/17.5.485.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Nico E, Daprati E, Nighoghossian N, Carrier E, Duhamel J-R, Sirigu A: The role of the right parietal lobe in anorexia nervosa. Psychol Med. 2010, 40: 1531-1539. 10.1017/S0033291709991851.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  20. Tomasino SJ: Does right parietal cortex and vestibular dysfunction underlie body image distortion?. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1996, 184: 758-

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  21. Daprati E, Sirigu A, Nico D: Body and movement: consciousness in the parietal lobes. Neuropsychologia. 2010, 48: 756-762. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.10.008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Decety J, Grèzes J: The power of simulation: imagining one’s own and other's behavior. Brain Res. 2006, 1079: 4-14. 10.1016/j.brainres.2005.12.115.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  23. Hesslow G: Conscious thought as simulation of behaviour and perception. Trends Cogn Sci. 2002, 6: 242-247. 10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01913-7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Warren WH, Whang S: Visual guidance of walking through apertures: body-scaled information for affordances. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1987, 13: 371-383.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Morgado N, Muller D, Gentaz E, Palluel-Germain R: Close to me? The influence of affective closeness on space perception. Perception. 2011, 40: 877-879. 10.1068/p6830.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Almeida QJ, Lebold CA: Freezing of gait in Parkinson’s disease: a perceptual cause for a motor impairment?. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2010, 81: 513-518. 10.1136/jnnp.2008.160580.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  27. Cohen RG, Chao A, Nutt JG, Horak FB: Freezing of gait is associated with a mismatch between motor imagery and motor execution in narrow doorways, not with failure to judge doorway passability. Neuropsychologia. 2011, 49: 3981-3988. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.10.014.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Anderson DA, Lundgren JD, Shapiro JR, Paulosky C: Assessment of eating disorders: review and recommendations for clinical use. Behav Modif. 2004, 28: 763-782. 10.1177/0145445503259851.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Cooper PJ, Taylor MJ, Cooper Z, Fairburn CG: The development and validation of the body shape questionnaire. Int J Eat Disord. 1987, 6: 485-494. 10.1002/1098-108X(198707)6:4<485::AID-EAT2260060405>3.0.CO;2-O.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Garner DM: Psychological assessment ressources. Eating Disorder Inventory-2. 1991

    Google Scholar 

  31. Gardner RM, Brown DL: Body image assessment: a review of figural drawing scales. Pers Individ Dif. 2010, 48: 107-111. 10.1016/j.paid.2009.08.017.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Gardner RM, Jappe LM, Gardner L: Development and validation of a new figural drawing scale for body-image assessment: the BIAS-BD. J Clin Psychol. 2009, 65: 113-123. 10.1002/jclp.20526.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Wolpert DM, Ghahramani Z: Computational principles of movement neuroscience. Nat Neurosci. 2000, 3 Suppl (november): 1212-1217.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  34. Frith CD, Blakemore SJ, Wolpert DM: Abnormalities in the awareness and control of action. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2000, 355: 1771-1788. 10.1098/rstb.2000.0734.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Franchak JM, Van der Zalm DJ, Adolph KE: Learning by doing: action performance facilitates affordance perception. Vision Res. 2010, 50: 2758-2765. 10.1016/j.visres.2010.09.019.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Riva G, Gaudio S: Allocentric lock in anorexia nervosa: new evidences from neuroimaging studies. Med Hypothesis. 2012, 79: 113-117. 10.1016/j.mehy.2012.03.036.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. American Psychiatric Association: The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders- Text Revision. 2000, Washington DC: American Psychiatric Publishing, 4

    Google Scholar 

  38. Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Amorim P, Janavs J, Weiller E, Hergueta T, Baker RDG: The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J Clin Psychiatry. 1998, 59: 34-57.

    Google Scholar 

  39. Wilmut K, Barnett AL: Locomotor adjustments when navigating through apertures. Hum Mov Sci. 2010, 29: 289-298. 10.1016/j.humov.2010.01.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Cash TF, Counts B, Hangen J, Huffine C: How much do you weigh? Determinant of validity of self reported body weight. Percept Mot Skills. 1989, 69: 248-250. 10.2466/pms.1989.69.1.248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Ellison Z, Jackie F, Howard R, Bullmore E, Williams S, Treasure J: Functional anatomy of calorie fear in anorexia nervosa. Lancet. 1998, 352: 1192-

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  42. Tchanturia K, Anderluh MB, Morris RG, Rabe-Hesketh S, Collier DA, Sanchez P, Treasure JL: Cognitive flexibility in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2004, 10: 513-520. 10.1017/S1355617704104086.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Abbate-Daga G, Buzzichelli S, Amianto F, Rocca G, Marzola E, McClintock SM, Fassino S: Cognitive flexibility in verbal and nonverbal domains and decision making in anorexia nervosa patients: a pilot study. BMC Psychiatry. 2011, 11: 162-10.1186/1471-244X-11-162.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Bastiani AM, Rao R, Weltzin T, Kaye WH: Perfectionism in anorexia nervosa. Int J Eat Disord. 1995, 17: 147-152. 10.1002/1098-108X(199503)17:2<147::AID-EAT2260170207>3.0.CO;2-X.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  45. Boeren A: Master's thesis Tillburg. Perfectionism in Anorexia Nervosa: A Literature Review. 2013, (July):0–40

    Google Scholar 

  46. Grunwald M, Weiss T: Inducing sensory stimulation in treatment of anorexia nervosa. Q J Med. 2005, 98: 379-380. 10.1093/qjmed/hci061.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  47. Vancampfort D, Vanderlinden J, De Hert M, Adámkova M, Skjaerven LH, Catalán-Matamoros D, Lundvik-Gyllensten A, Gómez-Conesa A, Ijntema R, Probst M: A systematic review on physical therapy interventions for patients with binge eating disorder. Disabil Rehabil. 2013, 35: 2191-2196. 10.3109/09638288.2013.771707.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Normand J-M, Giannopoulos E, Spanlang B, Slater M: Multisensory stimulation can induce an illusion of larger belly size in immersive virtual reality. PLoS One. 2011, 6: e16128-10.1371/journal.pone.0016128.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  49. Marco JH, Perpiñá C, Botella C: Effectiveness of cognitive behavioral therapy supported by virtual reality in the treatment of body image in eating disorders: one year follow-up. Psychiatry Res. 2013, 209: 619-625. 10.1016/j.psychres.2013.02.023.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The present work was funded by the CNRS and by a Bonus Quality Research Grant (University of Lille 3). We thank the study participants for their participation.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Marion Luyat.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

MM: participated in the design of the experiment, carried out the experiment and drafted the manuscript. DG: conceived and designed the experiment in equal contribution with ML, helped to draft the manuscript, participated of the recruitment of patients. IB: helped to carry out the experiment and helped to draft the manuscript. GL: participated in the design of the study, helped to draft the manuscript. MG: helped to draft the manuscript. OC: participated in the recruitment of the patients, helped to draft the manuscript. ML: conception and design of the experiment in equal contribution with DG, drafted the manuscript in equal contribution with MM. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Rights and permissions

Open Access  This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made.

The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder.

To view a copy of this licence, visit https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (https://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Metral, M., Guardia, D., Bauwens, I. et al. Painfully thin but locked inside a fatter body: abnormalities in both anticipation and execution of action in anorexia nervosa. BMC Res Notes 7, 707 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1186/1756-0500-7-707

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1756-0500-7-707

Keywords