Skip to main content

Automatic activation of alcohol cues by child maltreatment related words: a replication attempt in a different treatment setting

Abstract

Background

A growing body of research attempts to clarify the underlying mechanisms of the association between emotional maltreatment and alcohol dependence (AD). In a preceding study, we found considerable support for a specific priming effect in subjects with AD and emotional abuse experiences receiving alcohol rehabilitation treatment. We concluded that maltreatment related cues can automatically activate an associative memory network comprising cues eliciting craving as well as alcohol-related responses. Generalizability of the results to other treatment settings remains unclear because of considerable differences in German treatment settings as well as insufficiently clarified influences of selection effects. As replication studies in other settings are necessary, the current study aimed to replicate the specific priming effect in a qualified detoxification sample.

Results

22 AD subjects (n = 10 with emotional abuse vs. n = 12 without emotional abuse) participated in a priming experiment. Comparison data from 34 healthy control subjects were derived from the prior study. Contrary to our hypothesis, we did not find a specific priming effect.

Conclusions

We could not replicate the result of an automatic network activation by maltreatment related words in a sample of subjects with AD and emotional abuse experiences receiving qualified detoxification treatment. This discrepancy might be attributed to reasons related to treatment settings as well as to methodological limitations. Future work is required to determine the generalizability of the specific priming effect before valid conclusions regarding automatic activation can be drawn.

Background

Recent research indicates that there is a link between child maltreatment and alcohol dependence (AD; [1, 2]) and predominantly between emotional types of maltreatment and AD [35]. However, the underlying mechanisms still need to be clarified. Findings of laboratory-based experimental investigations suggest that trauma-related cues are able to increase craving, presumably by eliciting negative emotions [6, 7]. Based on memory network models [8], it can be assumed that the processes underlying the induction of craving by trauma-related cues are very fast, automatic and largely unconscious. Trauma relevant cues might automatically activate the memory network thus stimulating craving [9]. Accordingly, Sherman [10] found that trauma exposed smokers showed a significant priming effect within a modified Stroop task, when trauma related pictures preceded smoking related words. There is reason to assume that maltreatment related cues automatically activate an associative memory network comprising cues eliciting craving as well as alcohol-related responses. To our knowledge, there is only one study examining this assumption so far [11], which was conducted by our group. Using a priming paradigm, we examined whether the associative memory network in subjects with AD and experiences of childhood emotional abuse could be activated by child maltreatment related cues. The AD sample consisted of 49 individuals who were receiving alcohol rehabilitation treatment in Germany. As expected, we found considerable support for a specific priming effect in the emotionally abused alcoholics. We concluded that cues related to child maltreatment can automatically activate the associative memory network in AD subjects with emotional abuse experiences.

Considering the selective sample of patients enrolled in long-term rehabilitation treatment along with the overall complexity of the German addiction treatment system, generalizability of the results remains unclear. The German addiction treatment system comprises several kinds of treatment settings with somatic detoxification treatment, qualified detoxification treatment and rehabilitation treatment being the most frequent. While somatic detoxification treatment focuses on physical withdrawal only, qualified detoxification treatment is predominantly considered to motivate patients to engage in a subsequent long-term rehabilitation treatment [12]. However, only a limited number of patients actually proceed to rehabilitative treatment with transition rates ranging from 4 to 21% [1316], suggesting considerable selection effects. Yet, studies are lacking which provide information about the extent and characteristics of potential selection effects by comparing patients participating in rehabilitation programs to those who decide against it. To our knowledge, there is only one report, which peripherally addressed this issue. Results indicated that patients proceeding to rehabilitation had a more obsessive consumption pattern, more negative consequences of drinking and a more severe psychopathological burden as compared to subjects quitting after detoxification [17]. Altogether, differences in treatment settings as well as influences of selection effects require clarification.

Against this background, it remains unclear, if the finding of the automatic activation of the associative memory network by child maltreatment related cues can be generalized to treatment settings other than rehabilitation treatment. The present study aimed to replicate the finding in patients undergoing qualified detoxification therapy after having completed physical withdrawal. We presumed that the automatic network activation occurs independently of treatment setting. Hence, we proposed to find the priming effect of maltreatment related words on alcohol words in a qualified detoxification sample, as well. Analogous to our prior work, we used socially threatening cues as well as physically threatening cues to differentiate between emotional and physical maltreatment. As we assume that stimuli associated with physical abuse are integrated in the same associative network as stimuli associated with emotional abuse, we expected to find the priming effect in subjects with emotional abuse experiences for words related to emotional abuse as well as for words related to physical abuse.

Methods

Participants

The AD sample consisted of 27 subjects receiving short-term treatment in a German day-unit immediately after finishing physical detoxification. For inclusion in the study, AD participants in both samples had to be aged over 18 and had to meet DSM-IV criteria for AD [18] as principal diagnosis. Exclusion criteria were current comorbid substance use disorder, current or lifetime psychosis, and severe cognitive problems. Patients meeting inclusion criteria were asked to participate in the study at the beginning of their treatment. Interested patients received a complete description of the study and were invited to the clinical interview. All participants of the interview, who were native German speakers without neurological problems or dyslexia, were asked to participate additionally in the experimental study. Interested subjects received a description of the experimental design and an appointment was scheduled within seven days after the interview. Overall, five AD participants had to be excluded. Three participants did not reach the cut-off score in the AUDIT and two participants turned out to have cannabis dependence as principal diagnosis. The whole sample (n = 22) was divided into two subsamples according to the presence of emotional abuse (AD + EA vs. AD + no EA) based on the subscale emotional abuse of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ; [19]; see below).

Comparison data from 34 healthy control subjects were derived from a previously published study [11]. Table 1 provides an overview of the socio-demographic and clinical characteristics of the AD participants with emotional abuse (n = 10), AD patients without emotional abuse (n = 12), and the control participants (n = 34) as well as significant group differences. The control group was significantly younger. However, there were no differences between the two patients groups.

Table 1 Subjects’ sociodemographic and clinical characteristics

Power analysis suggests that a total sample size of N = 48 is needed to replicate the priming effect with an expected medium effect size of f = 0.30 assuming a power of ≥0.80 and an alpha level of 0.05 (G*Power, Version 3.1.9.2, University Düsseldorf, Germany).

Procedure and materials

A detailed report of the procedure and the materials has been published elsewhere [11]. All subjects who provided written informed consent underwent a clinical examination carried out by experienced clinical psychologists. The clinical examination included both a standardized interview and self-report measures. The German version of the AUDIT [20] was used for the assessment of participants’ level of alcohol use. The presence of a substance use disorder diagnosis was assessed with section E of the German version of the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders (SCID-I; [21]. The German version of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ; [19]) was administered to measure different types of childhood maltreatment occurring in the family context. The retrospective self-report questionnaire differentiates between emotional abuse, emotional neglect, physical abuse, physical neglect and sexual abuse. Parts of SCID-I were used to assess relevant psychiatric diagnoses. Besides section E (substance use disorders), the entire section A (mood disorders) and parts of section F (anxiety disorders: panic disorder, agoraphobia without history of panic disorder, agoraphobia with panic disorder, social phobia and generalized anxiety disorder) were used. DSM-IV diagnosis and severity of PTSD were assessed using the German version of the posttraumatic stress diagnostic scale (PDS; [22]).

The experimental task was identical for both samples and consisted of the priming task and the SAM-rating. AD subjects meeting inclusion criteria participated in the experimental task in an additional session. Control subjects participated in the experimental task immediately after the clinical examination. In the priming task, participants had to indicate after the presentation of a prime word whether the target was a real or a nonsense word by pressing the corresponding key. All real word stimuli consisted of German words from different categories: socially threatening words (SOC), physically threatening words (PHYS), alcohol related words (ALC) and neutral words (NEU). The priming task consisted of 16 practice trials and 192 experimental trials. Pairing of prime and target words resulted in eight stimulus conditions, which are outlined in Table 2. The priming task was immediately followed by the manipulation check. Participants were asked to rate all stimuli except the nonsense words regarding emotional valence and arousal using the self-assessment manikin self-report scale [23]. Further procedure details regarding the experimental design can be found in Potthast et al. [11].

Table 2 Stimulus conditions in the priming task

The study was approved by the Ethical Review Board of the University of Bielefeld. Control subjects were financially compensated for their participation.

Data analysis

All statistical analyses are identical to those analyses used in our prior study [11]. Group comparisons in terms of demographic and psychometric properties were performed using χ2-tests and analyses of variance (ANOVA). The SAM ratings regarding valence and arousal were analysed with repeated-measures ANOVAs with word category (SOC, PHYS, ALC, NEU) as within-subject variable and group (AD + EA, AD + no EA, no AD) as between-subject variables. Bonferroni adjusted post hoc comparisons were used to compare word categories within each group. Differences in priming between the groups were tested using repeated measures analyses of covariance (ANCOVA) with age as covariate, separately calculated for SOC and PHYS. In both conditions, separate ANCOVAs were conducted comparing the test trials (SOC-ALC or PHYS-ALC) to the baseline trials with a neutral target (SOC-NEU or PHYS-NEU) and to the baseline trials with a neutral prime (NEU-ALC). With respect to our hypothesis of a specific priming effect, we expected a significant interaction between stimulus condition and group. In case of a significant interaction effect, the three groups were compared using an ANCOVA with planned comparisons (AD + EA vs. the other two subgroups). Difference scores (test trials minus baseline trials) were used as dependent variables, age was used as covariate.

Results

Self-assessment manikin

For the valence rating, the repeated measures ANOVA showed a significant main effect for word category, F (3, 51) = 45.71; p < 0.0001; η 2 = 0.73, a significant interaction of word category and group, F (6, 102) = 6.41; p < 0.0001; η 2 = 0.27, but no main effect of group, F (2, 53) = 2.38; p = 0.103; η 2 = 0.08. Regarding arousal, the ANOVA revealed a significant main effect for word category, F (3, 51) = 43.65; p < 0.0001; η 2 = 0.72, as well as a significant word category x group interaction, F (6, 102) = 6.84; p < 0.0001; η 2 = 0.29, but no main effect of group F (2, 53) = 2.81; p = 0.069; η 2 = 0.10. Bonferroni adjusted post hoc tests showed that within each group the socially threatening words as well as the physically threatening words were rated significantly more negative than neutral words (all p values <0.001). Additionally, within each group socially and physically threatening words were rated as more arousing than the neutral words. All p values were less than 0.01, except for the socially threatening words in the no AD group (p = 1.00). The manipulation check approved that socially and physically threatening cues elicited emotional reactions of the expected value.

Priming task

The results of the ANCOVAS regarding the socially threatening words are illustrated in Fig. 1. Table 3 reports the corresponding mean response time and standard error in the test and baseline trials. As opposed to the hypothesis, the ANCOVA showed no significant effects when comparing the test trials to the neutral prime baseline [stimulus condition x group: F (2, 52) = 0.22; p = 0.807; η 2 = 0.01; stimulus condition: F (1, 52) = 0.03; p = 0.869; η 2 = 0.00; group: F (2, 52) = 2.47; p = 0.094; η 2 = 0.09]. Similarly, no significant effects were found when comparing the test trials to the neutral target baseline [stimulus condition x group: F (2, 52) = 0.57; p = 0.568; η 2 = 0.02; stimulus condition: F (1, 52) = 1.51; p = 0.224; η 2 = 0.03; group: F (2, 52) = 1.65; p = 0.202; η 2 = 0.06].

Fig. 1
figure 1

Average response time on socially threatening test trial (SOC-ALC) as contrasted to baseline trial with a neutral prime (NEU-ALC) on the left and to baseline trial with a neutral target (SOC-NEU) on the right. AD + EA subjects with alcohol dependence and emotional abuse experiences; AD + no EA subjects with alcohol dependence without emotional abuse experiences; no AD control subjects; SOC socially threatening words; ALC alcohol related words; NEU neutral words

Table 3 Means and standard errors for response time in milliseconds on test and baseline trials

The results of the ANCOVAS concerning the physically threatening words are shown in Fig. 2 and the corresponding descriptive statistics in Table 3. Analogous to the socially threatening words, we found no significant effects when comparing the test trials to the neutral prime baseline (stimulus condition x group: F (2, 52) = 0.03; p = 0.975; η 2 = 0.00; stimulus condition: F (1, 52) = 0.547; p = 0.463; η 2 = 0.01; group: F (2, 52) = 2.59; p = 0.084; η 2 = 0.09). Similarly, the ANCOVA showed no significant effects when comparing the test trials to the neutral target baseline (stimulus condition x group: F (2, 52) = 0.64; p = 0.533; η 2 = 0.02; stimulus condition: F (1, 52) = 2.30; p = 0.135; η 2 = 0.04; group: F (2, 53) = 2.15; p = 0.127; η 2 = 0.08). As we did not find any significant interaction effects, no planned comparisons were conducted.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Average response time on physically threatening test trial (PHYS-ALC) as contrasted to baseline trial with a neutral prime (NEU-ALC) on the left and to baseline trial with a neutral target (PHYS-NEU) on the right. AD + EA subjects with alcohol dependence and emotional abuse experiences; AD + no EA subjects with alcohol dependence without emotional abuse experiences; no AD control subjects; PHYS physically threatening words; ALC alcohol related words; NEU neutral words

Discussion

The purpose of the present study was to replicate our finding of a specific priming effect of maltreatment related words on alcohol words in a qualified detoxification sample. Our preceding study [11] indicated that only the AD + EA group showed a considerable reduction in response time to alcohol related target words after the presentation of maltreatment related prime words. The other two groups showed no reduction in response time. We concluded that the associative memory network can automatically be activated by child maltreatment related cues in subjects with AD and emotional abuse experiences. By means of the same priming paradigm we examined whether this automatic activation can also be found in subjects with AD and emotional abuse experiences being in qualified detoxification treatment instead of rehabilitation treatment. Interestingly, this pattern of results could not be replicated in qualified detoxification setting, as we did not find a specific priming effect in the present study.

This interesting finding of fundamentally different results within two distinct treatment settings deserves special emphasis. On the one hand, this discrepancy might be attributed to actually existing differences between both samples concerning the automatic activation of the associative network by child maltreatment related cues. This would be consistent to the possible selection effects regarding treatment settings, which are suggested by small transition rates from detoxification treatment to long-term rehabilitation treatment [1316]. Weithmann and Hoffmann [17] indicated that patients proceeding to rehabilitation treatment seem to be more severely affected by their AD. They reported a more obsessive consumption pattern, more negative consequences of drinking and a more severe psychopathological burden as opposed to patients who quit after detoxification. Considering this finding and the fact that the specific priming effect was solely found within the rehabilitation setting, it might be speculated that the highly automatic activation of the associative network by child maltreatment related cues is closely related to AD severity. However, when compared to the rehabilitation sample in our prior study the present qualified detoxification sample does not show consistently higher levels of symptoms and impairments, in particular with respect to the AUDIT score as well as to comorbid psychopathology. Besides AD severity, the discrepancy could be caused by differences concerning the current treatment focus. Whereas detoxification treatment and especially qualified detoxification treatment predominantly aims at motivating patients to engage in a subsequent long-term rehabilitation treatment [12], patients being in rehabilitative treatment have to deal intensively with their drinking motives and triggers for craving. It is conceivable, that such therapeutic work might affect central elements of the associative memory network, thereby resulting in an initial reinforcement of the network associations. Additionally, the divergence of findings could be ascribed to alcohol related cognitive deficits, which probably differ in severity between qualified detoxification patients and those adhering to rehabilitation programs. It might be speculated that during the early qualified detoxification phase, cognitive functioning is more globally impaired thereby disguising a potential priming effect. Although cognitive deficits during both short- and long-term abstinence are well known [24], evidence regarding the rate of cognitive recovery is currently contradictory. Moreover, there are large interindividual differences in recovery process [25] impeding conclusions. Cognitive processes might also have been influenced by psychopharmacological medication for AD such as acamprosate and naltrexone, which was found in 30% of the qualified detoxification subjects but not within the rehabilitation sample of the prior study. As literature shows a significant reduction of cue-reactivity by acamprosate and naltrexone [2628], a possible priming-effect might be disguised by such a medication. Due to the small sample size, a reanalysis of data after exclusion of qualified detoxifications subjects receiving AD medication was unfortunately not feasible.

Next to reasons related to treatment setting, the rather unexpected findings in the present study might also be attributed to methodological limitations. Firstly, the discrepancy might be assigned to the small sample size of the qualified detoxification subsamples (n = 10 and n = 12). Nevertheless, effect sizes for interactions between stimulus condition and group ranging from 0.00 to 0.02 are indicative that the absence of a priming effect does not stem from a lack of statistical power. Anyway, replication studies with larger qualified detoxification samples are warranted. Secondly, it has to be considered, that the qualified detoxification sample is drawn only from one institution, which is a day-unit institution in addition. Thus, a multicentric approach is preferable to evaluate the generalizability of findings for qualified detoxification services in general. Moreover, it remains to be clarified, how findings are generalizable to somatic detoxification treatments as the proportion of patients proceeding to qualified detoxification is limited. It would be particularly interesting to know, if findings are applicable to those patients, who quit after somatic detoxification. Albeit, the evaluation of a specific priming effect in a somatic detoxification sample would be hard to realize because of confounding factors. Finally, the unexpected findings in the present study might be attributed to a general vulnerability of priming effects regarding their replicability. In fact, a number of authors have found priming effects to be highly sensitive to variations in experimental features and subject populations [29, 30].

In summary we can say that additional work is required to determine the generalizability of the specific priming effect of maltreatment related words in patients with AD as suggested by our previous study [11]. Replication studies in different treatment settings, with multicentric samples and with large sample sizes are needed to clarify reasons for the discrepant results in the present study, for instance by considering potential confounding factors, such as AD severity or level of alcohol-related or medication-related cognitive deficits.

At the current stage, we cannot draw valid conclusions concerning automatic activation of the associative memory network as a potential underlying mechanism of the link between child maltreatment and AD. The present results show that prior evidence arguing for the existence of such a mechanism might be associated to the specific treatment setting of the study sample. Further research is needed to evaluate the robustness and generalizability of related findings across treatment settings, treatment institutions and populations thereby allowing important implications regarding the treatment of AD.

Abbreviations

AD:

alcohol dependence

EA:

emotional abuse

References

  1. Young-Wolff K, Kendler K, Ericson M, Prescott C. Accounting for the association between childhood maltreatment and alcohol-use disorders in males: a twin study. Psychol Med. 2011;41(1):59–70.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Singh VAS, Thornton T, Tonmyr L. Determinants of substance abuse in a population of children and adolescents involved with the child welfare system. Int J Mental Health Addict. 2011;9(4):382–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Potthast N, Neuner F, Catani C. The contribution of emotional maltreatment to alcohol dependence in a treatment-seeking sample. Addict Behav. 2014;39:949–58.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Rosenkranz SE, Muller RT, Henderson JL. Psychological maltreatment in relation to substance use problem severity among youth. Child Abuse Negl. 2012;36:438–48.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Schwandt ML, Heilig M, Hommer DW, George DT, Ramchandani VA. Childhood trauma exposure and alcohol dependence severity in adulthood: mediation by emotional abuse severity and neuroticism. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2013;37(6):984–92.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Coffey SF, Schumacher JA, Stasiewicz PR, Henslee AM, Baillie LE, Landy N. Craving and physiological reactivity to trauma and alcohol cues in posttraumatic stress disorder and alcohol dependence. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2010;18(4):340–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Coffey SF, Stasiewicz PR, Hughes PM, Brimo ML. Trauma-focused imaginal exposure for individuals with comorbid posttraumatic stress disorder and alcohol dependence: revealing mechanisms of alcohol craving in a cue reactivity paradigm. Psychol Addict Behav. 2006;20(4):425–35.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Baker TB, Morse E, Sherman JE. The motivation to use drugs: a psychobiological analysis of urges. In: Rivers PC, editor. The nebraska symposium on motivation: alcohol use and abuse. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press; 1987. p. 257–323.

    Google Scholar 

  9. Stewart SH, Pihl RO, Conrod PJ, Dongier M. Functional associations among trauma, PTSD, and substance-related disorders. Addict Behav. 1998;23(6):797–812.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Sherman PB Trauma-based priming and attentional bias to smoking cues: a Stroop task study. University of South Florida. 2007. http://scholarcommons.usf.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=5002&context=etd. Accessed 13 Apr 2016.

  11. Potthast N, Neuner F, Catani C. When abuse primes addiction—automatic activation of alcohol concepts by child maltreatment related cues in emotionally abused alcoholics. Addict Behav. 2015;48:62–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Mann KF. Neue ärztliche Aufgaben bei Alkoholproblemen [New tasks of physicians to over-come alcohol problems]. Deutsches Ärzteblatt. 2002;99(10):632–44.

    Google Scholar 

  13. Lange W, Veltrup C, Driessen M. Entgiftung und qualifizierte Entzugsbehandlung: Kurz-, mittel- und langfristige Katamnesen [Short medical detoxification vs. withdrawal treatment: Short-term, middle-term and long-term follow-ups]. Sucht. 1999;45(4):228–34.

    Google Scholar 

  14. Bauer U, Hasenöhrl A. Therapieerfolg Alkoholabhängiger nach qualifizierter Entzugsbehandlung und konventioneller Entgiftung (vergleichende 28-Monats-Katamnese) [A comparison of an alcohol withdrawal treatment program with short medical detoxification (Follow-up results from 28 months)]. SUCHT-Zeitschrift für Wissenschaft und Praxis/J Addict Res Pract. 2000;46(4):250–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Reymann G, Danziger H. Replacing the last week of a motivational inpatient alcohol withdrawal programme by a day-clinic setting. Eur Addict Res. 2001;7(2):56–60.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Zielke M. Krankheitsverläufe von Versicherten mit aktuellen alkoholbedingten Intoxikationen auf der Basis von Sekundärdaten der DAK [Progress of disease of insurants with current alcohol intoxication on the basis of secondary data of the German health insurance DAK]. Sucht Aktuell. 2006;13(1):13.

    Google Scholar 

  17. Weithmann G, Hoffmann M. Vermittlung in Entwöhnung nach Alkoholentzugsbehandlung [Successful placement of patients into long-term rehabilitation following alcohol detoxification]. SUCHT-Zeitschrift für Wissenschaft und Praxis/J Addict Res Pract. 2006;52(5):297–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Association AP. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-IV-TR. Arlington: American Psychiatric Publishing Inc.; 2000.

    Google Scholar 

  19. Wingenfeld K, Spitzer C, Mensebach C, Grabe HJ, Hill A, Gast U, Schlosser N, Höpp H, Beblo T, Driessen M. Die deutsche Version des Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ): Erste Befunde zu den psychometrischen Kennwerten [The German version of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ): Preliminary psychometric properties]. Psychother Psychosom Med Psychol. 2010;60(11):442–50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Rumpf H, Meyer C, Hapke U, John U. Deutsche Version des Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT) [German Version of the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT)]. Elektronisches Handbuch zu Erhebungsinstrumenten im Suchtbereich (EHES) 2001.

  21. Wittchen HU, Zaudig M, Fydrich T. SKID. Strukturiertes Klinisches Interview für DSM-IV-Achse I und II [SCID. Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV axis I and II disorders]. Göttingen: Hogrefe; 1997.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Steil R, Ehlers A. Die Posttraumatische Diagnoseskala (PDS) [The Posttraumatic Stress Diagnostic Scale (PDS)]. Jena: Universität Jena, Psychologisches Institut; 2000.

    Google Scholar 

  23. Bradley MM, Lang PJ. Measuring emotion: the self-assessment manikin and the semantic differential. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 1994;25(1):49–59.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Stavro K, Pelletier J, Potvin S. Widespread and sustained cognitive deficits in alcoholism: a meta-analysis. Addict Biol. 2013;18(2):203–13.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Bates ME, Buckman JF, Nguyen TT. A role for cognitive rehabilitation in increasing the effectiveness of treatment for alcohol use disorders. Neuropsychol Rev. 2013;23(1):27–47.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Rohsenow DJ, Monti PM, Hutchison KE, Swift RM, Colby SM, Kaplan GB. Naltrexone’s effects on reactivity to alcohol cues among alcoholic men. J Abnorm Psychol. 2000;109(4):738–42.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Palfai T, Davidson D, Swift R. Influence of naltrexone on cue-elicited craving among hazardous drinkers: the moderational role of positive outcome expectancies. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 1999;7(3):266–73.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Weinstein A, Feldtkeller B, Feeney A, Lingford-Hughes A, Nutt D. A pilot study on the effects of treatment with acamprosate on craving for alcohol in alcohol-dependent patients. Addict Biol. 2003;8(2):229–32.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Cesario J. Priming, replication, and the hardest science. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2014;9(1):40–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Klatzky RL, Creswell JD. An intersensory interaction account of priming effects—and their absence. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2014;9(1):49–58.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors’ contributions

All authors have participated substantially in the conception and design of the study. NP collected data, conducted the statistical analysis and interpretation of findings and drafted the manuscript. FN made substantial contributions to the interpretation of findings and revised the manuscript. CC made substantial contributions to the statistical analyses and interpretations of findings, and helped to draft and revise the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to all participants who took part in the study. The authors thank the staff of the collaborating clinics for supporting recruitment of participants and Yasemin Coskun and Lena Bergmann for helping with data collection.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Availability of data and materials

Data are available upon request from the first author.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study was approved by the Ethical Review Board of the University of Bielefeld (Protocol #2011004).

Funding

We acknowledge support for the Article Processing Charge by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft and the Open Access Publication Fund of Bielefeld University.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Nadine Potthast.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Potthast, N., Neuner, F. & Catani, C. Automatic activation of alcohol cues by child maltreatment related words: a replication attempt in a different treatment setting. BMC Res Notes 10, 17 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13104-016-2324-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13104-016-2324-8

Keywords