- Research note
- Open Access
- Published:
Narrative elaboration makes misinformation and corrective information regarding COVID-19 more believable
BMC Research Notes volumeĀ 15, ArticleĀ number:Ā 235 (2022)
Abstract
Objective
People gather information about health topics from online channels oftentimes awash with misinformation. Investigating this problem during the COVID-19 pandemic is important, as the misinformation effect occurs when misleading details are embedded in narratives and questions. This pilot study investigated whether narrative elaboration increases believability in misinformation statements about COVID-19, and willingness to share these statements online.
Results
Results from our online survey (nā=ā80) demonstrated that narrative elaboration increased believability in both misinformation and accurate statements, with a more pronounced effect on younger adults. Future research may investigate cognitive vulnerabilities imposed by elaborate narratives embedded in online health misinformation with increased attention on developing misinformation resilience among younger adults.
Introduction
Misinformation is broadly understood an umbrella term to include all false or inaccurate information that is spread in social media [1]. Since the outbreak of the SARS-CoV-2 virus in 2020 online platforms have been awash with COVID-19 pandemic-related information, however these are often abound with misinformation containing content which is misleading or false [2] (see [3] for an evaluation of online source credibility). Thus, the influence of misinformation is highly impactive as differences in believability between credible content and misinformation can cause a split between scientific consensus and public opinion [4]. Refutation text, such as explicit rebuttal of misleading content, can reduce belief in misinformation [5]. However, when added detail elaborates on misinformation, this additive content can increase suggestibility to a greater extent than detail that contradicts the misinformation. For example, Huff and Umanath [6] asked participants to read fictional narratives and answer questions accompanied with additive and contradictory misinformation statements. Questions about the narratives that were accompanied with additive misinformation were endorsed more so than narratives with contradictory misinformation, even with the participants were explicitly advised about and instructed to identify errors in the text. Similarly, Pennycook, Cannon, and Rand [7], incorporating a fake-news paradigm, found that participants still endorsed fake-news articles as accurate even when they had been labelled as contested by fact-checkers. Thus, elaborating on misinformation statements can make them more believable, whilst flagging misinformation as disputed is less effective in reducing believability.
The spread of misinformation is also implicated by individualsā willingness to share knowledge [8, 9], especially if the validity of the information is not challenged. Pennycook, McPhetres, Zhang, Lu, and Rand [10] found greater sharing of online misinformation about COVID-19 if participants were not questioned as to its accuracy. Vijaykumar et al. [11] also found that COVID-19 statements that were only partially true increased belief in misinformation. Furthermore younger adults showed a greater proclivity to sharing misinformation than older adults, likely due to greater social media use in order to source health information [12] and lower levels of eHealth literacy [13]. In contrast, Greenspan and Loftus [14] found that older adults were more likely to share pandemic-related misinformation than younger adults, as older adults may have more trouble discerning the false information. Older adults more commonly use social media for socializing purposes than sourcing information, which suggests a lesser focus on the accuracy of the content [15]. Thus, age effects regarding sharing misinformation are inconclusive. However, corrective information appears to negate these inconsistencies by increasing intention by both younger and older adults age groups to share factual information about COVID-19 [11].
Building on this evidence, the data we present is a pilot study investigating the following research questions: (1) to what extent does misinformation with narrative elaboration about COVID-19 affect believability in misinformation; (2) to what extent does misinformation with narrative elaboration about COVID-19 affect willingness to share information online; (3) to what extent does the effect of misinformation with narrative elaboration about COVID-19 differ by age. We hypothesized that narrative elaboration would increase believability and willingness to share misinformation about COVID-19, in particular in younger adults.
Main text
Method
Participants
The sample consisted of 80 participants aged 18Ā years and over (range 18ā74Ā years, Mā=ā27.71, SDā=ā12.74), with 63 females, 16 males, and 1 participant who identified as non-binary. There were no other exclusion criteria. Participants were recruited online via social media, SurveySwap, and SurveyCircle. Males and females did not differ significantly in age, t(77)ā=ā0.493, pā=ā0.624. Participants were also grouped by age (youngerā=ā18ā34Ā years/olderā=ā35Ā yearsā+ā; see [16, 17]). The younger groupās mean age was significantly lower than the older adultsā, t(78)ā=āā ā23.96, pā<ā0.001 (see Table 1).
Materials
An 80-item bespoke questionnaire was developed for this study (seeĀ Supplementary Materials 1), containing COVID-19 related statements taken and fact-checked from the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (https://www.cdc.gov/) [18] and the World Health Organisation (https://www.who.int/) [19] websites. The questionnaire consisted of four conditions: misinformation-only, misinformation with narrative elaboration, truth-only, and truth with factual elaboration. Example items include: Garlic can cure COVID-19 (misinformation-only): Garlic can cure COVID-19. Garlic is a healthy food that may have some antimicrobial properties, helping to cure COVID (misinformation with elaboration); Antibiotics cannot prevent or treat COVID-19 (truth-only); Antibiotics cannot prevent or treat COVID-19. COVID-19 is caused by a virus, and therefore antibiotics should not be used for prevention or treatment (truth with elaboration). Participants read each statement and rated believability using a 1ā7 Likert scale where 1ā=ātotally disagree/7ā=ātotally agree, and their willingness to share the information where 1ā=āno intention at all share/7ā=āwill definitely share (Cronbachās αā=ā0.661).
Procedure
Ethical approval was granted by the Northumbria University Ethics Committee. Participants accessed the study via Qualtrics (www.qualtrics.com). After providing online consent, participants provided details of age and gender, and then responded to the COVID-19 statements and willingness to share questions.
Results
Data were analysed using SPSS, v28. Paired samples t-tests were conducted to determine differences in overall believability and willingness to share the statements. Independent samples t-tests were used to compare believability and willingness to share by Group. Correlation analyses investigated relationships between all variables while controlling for age (see Table 2).
Believability
Believability for misinformation-only statements was significantly lower (Mā=ā26.66, SDā=ā7.44) compared to misinformation with elaboration (Mā=ā27.86, SDā=ā8.61), t(79)ā=āāā2.60, pā=ā0.011, dā=ā0.15, and truth-only statements (Mā=ā45.01, SDā=ā10.89), t(79)ā=āāā12.99, pā<ā0.001, dā=ā1.97. Similarly, believability for misinformation with elaboration (Mā=ā27.86, SDā=ā8.61) was significantly lower than truth with elaboration (Mā=ā46.71, SDā=ā11.16), t(79)ā=āāā12.64, pā<ā0.001, dā=ā1.89. Believability for truth-only statements were also significantly lower than truth with elaboration statements, t(79)ā=āāā2.707, pā=ā0.008, dā=ā0.15.
Sharing intention
Participantsā willingness to share was significantly lower in response to truth-only (Mā=ā22.73, SDā=ā13.91) compared with truth with elaboration statements (Mā=ā24.51, SDā=ā15.29), t(79)ā=āāā3.09, pā=ā0.003, dā=ā0.12. Similarly, willingness to share misinformation-only statements (Mā=ā18.40, SDā=ā10.48) was significantly lower than truth-only statements (Mā=ā22.73, SDā=ā13.91), t(79)ā=āāā3.66, pā<ā0.001, dā=ā0.35. Willingness to share misinformation with elaboration (Mā=ā18.35, SDā=ā10.12) was significantly lower compared to truth with elaboration (Mā=ā24.51, SDā=ā15.29), t(79)ā=āāā4.49, pā<ā0.001, dā=ā0.46. There was no difference in willingness to share between misinformation-only and misinformation with elaboration statements, t(79)ā=ā0.128, pā=ā0.899, dā=ā0.005.
Believability and sharing intention by group
Independent samples t-tests revealed the younger participants gave significantly greater believability ratings compared to older adults to both misinformation-only, t(15.989)ā=ā1.745, pā=ā0.50, dā=ā0.56, and misinformation with elaboration statements, t(78)ā=ā2.207, pā=ā0.015, dā=ā0.62. All other analyses by Group were non-significant (pāā„ā0.057).
Correlations
In order to explore the relationship between believability, willingness to share, and age, a series of correlation and partial correlation (controlling for age) analyses were carried out (see Table 3). Believability and willingness to share were significantly positively correlated in response to all misinformation (pāā¤ā0.013) and truth (pāā¤ā0.008) statements, even when controlling for age (misinformation; pāā¤ā0.004: truth; pāā¤ā0.005). Significant negative correlations were observed between age and believability to misinformation-only (pā=ā0.007) and misinformation with elaboration (pā=ā0.004) statements only. All other analyses were non-significant (pāā„ā0.066).
Summary
Elaboration increased believability of both misinformation and true COVID-19 statements, however participants were equally willing to share misinformation-only and misinformation with narrative elaboration. Younger adultsā believability was significantly greater than older adults to both misinformation-only and misinformation with elaboration. Believability and willingness to share were significantly correlated in all conditions even when controlling for age.
Discussion
The aim of this pilot study was to investigate believability of and willingness to share misinformation statements about COVID-19 when supported with detailed explanation. Our study expands on the research reporting the effect of additive misinformation in the believability of incorrect information [6]. Here we add to this by demonstrating that adding narrative elaboration to misinformation statements about COVID-19 increases its believability also, especially in younger adults.
Both misinformation and true statements, when supported with detailed elaboration, were more believable than their misinformation-only and truth-only counterparts. Similar patterns were observed by Butterfuss and Kendeou [5] who demonstrated that the addition of factual elaboration to true statements increased believability, as this supplementary detail increased perceptions of itsā accuracy. Here we demonstrated this effect applies to misinformation with narrative elaboration about COVID-19 also. Truth-only and truth with elaboration statements were rated as more believable than both misinformation-only and misinformation with elaboration statements. This is consistent with prior research which also found that truth statements were more believable than misinformation statements in general [5]. However, when considering willingness to share the COVID-19 statements, participants were equally likely to share the misinformation-only and misinformation with elaboration statements. Research has also shown that individuals do not share misinformation with the goal of accuracy, and they are more willing to share when they are not questioned about the accuracy of the statements [10].
Whilst controlling for age did not alter correlations between believability and willingness to share, participant age was negatively correlated with believability for both misinformation-only and misinformation with elaboration statements, and significantly greater in the younger adult age group. This supports existing studies on COVID-19 misinformation and age [11] which also found that younger adults were more likely to believe misinformation than older adults. This is also in line with previous research showing older adults to be less susceptible to misinformation than younger adults [20]. Believability in misinformation is associated with repeated exposure [21] and younger individuals are known to spend more time on social media where COVID-19 misinformation is disseminated frequently [4]. Thus, the findings of the current study adds further understanding regarding the younger age-associated bias towards believability in COVID-19 misinformation. However, the existing literature regarding age and willingness to share misinformation is inconclusive [11, 14], and further emphasised here by the non-significant correlations.
Limitations
It is possible that the misinformation content in the current study did not align with participantsā existing opinions about COVID-19. Knowledge of COVID-19 is so widespread now that our misinformation statements may not have accurately tapped into misconceptions. Individuals are more likely to endorse misinformation if it is in line with their pre-existing beliefs [14]. Given that much COVID-19 misinformation is spread on social media, prior exposure to both accurate and misinformation was likely high for the participants [7]. A second key limitation was a lack of detail regarding the participantsā conspiracy theory and scientific beliefs (see [10, 21]). Distrust in conventional news outlets increases individualsā preference to source information from elsewhere such as online media that dissemminates misinformation [22]. Thus understanding of participantsā views regarding information sourcing would help to further elucidate the effect of narrative elaboration on intention to endorse misinformation. Finally, whilst this was a pilot study, the sample size was small. Replication of these findings with a more generalizable sample could have important implications for public health risk communication policy and practice.
Availability of data and materials
The data and materials for the study can be accessed via the Northumbria University Figshare service: https://figshare.northumbria.ac.uk/articles/dataset/Narrative_Elaboration_and_misinformation_regarding_COVID-19/19361453/1
References
Wu L, Morstatter F, Carley KM, Liu H. Misinformation in social media: definition, manipulation, and detection. ACM SIGKDD Explorations Newsl. 2020;21(2):80ā90.
Brennen JS, Simon F, Howard PN, Nielsen RK. Types, sources, and claims of COVID-19 misinformation. Reuters Institute. 2020;7:1ā13.
Broniatowski DA, Kerchner D, Farooq F, Huang X, Jamison AM, Dredze M, et al. Twitter and Facebook posts about COVID-19 are less likely to spread misinformation compared to other health topics. PLoS ONE. 2020;17(1): e0261768. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0261768.
Mian A, Khan S. Coronavirus: The spread of misinformation. BMC Med. 2020;18(1). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12916-020-01556-3
Butterfuss R, Kendeou P. Reducing interference from misconceptions: The role of inhibition in knowledge revision. J Educ Psych. 2020;112(4):782ā94. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000385.
Huff MJ, Umanath S. Evaluating suggestibility to additive and contradictory misinformation following explicit error detection in younger and older adults. J Exp Psychol Appl. 2018;24(2):180ā95. https://doi.org/10.1037/xap0000138.
Pennycook G, Cannon TD, Rand DG. Prior exposure increases perceived accuracy of fake news. SSRN Electron J. 2018. https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.2958246.
Grinberg N, Joseph K, Friedland L, Swire-Thompson B, Lazer D. Fake news on Twitter during the 2016 U.S. presidential election. Sci. 2019;363(6425):374ā378. doi:https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aau2706
Rich PR, Zaragoza MS. The continued influence of implied and explicitly stated misinformation in news reports. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2016;42(1):62ā74. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000155.
Pennycook G, McPhetres J, Zhang Y, Lu JG, Rand DG. Fighting COVID-19 misinformation on social media: Experimental evidence for a scalable Accuracy nudge intervention. Psych Sci. 2020;31(7):770ā80. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797620939054.
Vijaykumar S, Jin Y, Rogerson D, Lu X, Sharma S, Maughan A, et al. How shades of truth and age affect responses to COVID-19 (Mis)information: Randomized survey experiment among WhatsApp users in UK and Brazil. Humanit Soc Sci Commun. 2021;8(88). https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-021-00752-7
Prybutok G, Ryan S. Social media: the key to health information access for 18- to 30-year-old college students. Comput Inform Nurs. 2015;33(4):132ā41. PMID: 25887107. doi:https://doi.org/10.1097/CIN.0000000000000147.
Stellefson M, Hanik B, Chaney B, Chaney D, Tennant B, Chavarria EA. eHealth literacy among college students: a systematic review with implications for eHealth education. J Med Internet Res. 2011;13(4):e102.PubMed: 22155629. doi:https://doi.org/10.2196/jmir.1703
Greenspan RL, Loftus EF. Pandemics and infodemics: Research on the effects of misinformation on memory. Hum Beh Emerg Techn. 2021;3(1):8ā12. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbe2.228.
Brashier NM, Schacter DL. Aging in an era of fake news. Curr Direct Psychol Sci. 2020;29(3):316ā23. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721420915872.
Villanti AC, Johnson AL, Ilakkuvan V, Jacobs MA, Graham AL, Rath JM. Social media use and access to digital technology in US young adults in 2016. J Med Internet Res. 2017;19(6): e7303.
Lisitsa E, Benjamin KS, Chun SK, Skalisky J, Hammond LE, Mezulis AH. Loneliness among young adults during COVID-19 pandemic: The mediational roles of social media use and social support seeking. J Soc Clin Psychol. 2020;39(8):708ā26.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention https://www.cdc.gov/coronavirus/2019-nCoV/index.html. Accessed December 2020.
World Health Organisation https://www.who.int/emergencies/diseases/novel-coronavirus-2019. (Accessed Dec 2020).
Auslander MV, Thomas AK, Gutchess AH. Confidence moderates the role of control beliefs in the context of age-related changes in misinformation susceptibility. Exp Aging Res. 2017;43(3):305ā22. https://doi.org/10.1080/0361073x.2017.1298960.
Pan W, Liu D, Fang J. An Examination of Factors Contributing to the Acceptance of Online Health Misinformation. Front Psychol. 2021;12:630268. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.630268.
De Coninck D, Frissen T, Matthijs K, dāHaenens L, Lits G, Champagne-Poirier O, et al. Beliefs in conspiracy theories and misinformation about COVID-19: comparative perspectives on the role of anxiety, depression and exposure to and trust in information sources. Front Psychol. 2021;12:646394. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.646394.
Acknowledgements
Not applicable.
Funding
The study received no funding.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
All authors contributed equally to the conceptualisation and data analysis. KF collected the data and prepared the first draft. JG and SV contributed equally to the final manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Ethical approval and consent to participate
Ethical approval was granted by the Department of Psychology ethics committee, Northumbria University, UK. Participants provided online consent.
Consent for publication
Not applicable.
Competing interests
The authors declare there are no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Information
Additional file 1.
Supplementary Materials.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.
About this article
Cite this article
Greer, J., Fitzgerald, K. & Vijaykumar, S. Narrative elaboration makes misinformation and corrective information regarding COVID-19 more believable. BMC Res Notes 15, 235 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13104-022-06134-9
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13104-022-06134-9
Keywords
- Misinformation
- COVID-19
- Narrative elaboration
- Younger adults
- Older adults