Skip to main content
  • Research Note
  • Open access
  • Published:

Cross-cultural differences in self-reported and behavioural emotional self-awareness between Japan and the UK

Abstract

Objective

How we express and describe emotion is shaped by sociocultural norms. These sociocultural norms may also affect emotional self-awareness, i.e., how we identify and make sense of our own emotions. Previous studies have found lower emotional self-awareness in East Asian compared to Western samples using self-report measures. However, studies using behavioural methods did not provide clear evidence of reduced emotional self-awareness in East Asian groups. This may be due to different measurement tools capturing different facets of emotional self-awareness.

Results

To investigate this issue further, we compared the emotional self-awareness of Japanese (n = 29) and United Kingdom (UK) (n = 43) adults using the self-report Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20), alongside two behavioural measures – the Emotional Consistency Task (EC-Task) and the Photo Emotion Differentiation Task (PED-Task). Japanese adults showed higher TAS-20 scores than UK participants, indicating greater self-reported difficulties with emotional self-awareness. Japanese participants also had lower EC-Task scores than UK adults, indicating a lower ability to differentiate between levels of emotional intensity. PED-Task performance did not show clear group differences. These findings suggest that cross-cultural differences in emotional self-awareness vary with the task used, because different tasks assess distinct aspects of this ability. Future research should attempt to capture these different aspects of emotional self-awareness.

Peer Review reports

Introduction

Emotion is an integral part of our daily lives, driving socialisation, decision-making and well-being. Emotion is also shaped by culture; that is, the terms we use to describe emotion are learned through social interactions and shaped by cultural evolution [1]. Moreover, not all emotion terms exist in all languages and cultures [1]. Different cultures have different norms about how emotions should be expressed and how desirable certain emotions are considered [2]. Therefore, it appears likely that there may also be cultural differences in how people identify and describe their own emotions.

The ability to identify and describe our own emotions is likely to be fundamental to being able to manage and communicate them to others. However, defining and measuring emotional self-awareness is no easy task. Many constructs have emerged in the literature that attempt to capture emotional self-awareness, all with subtly different definitions and measurement methods [3]. In this study, we use ‘emotional self-awareness’ as an umbrella term to describe the general ability to identify, label and understand one’s own emotions. This term also includes a range of other constructs, such as emotional differentiation, i.e., the ability to differentiate between similar qualitative emotional states [4], and alexithymia [5], a clinical construct describing severe difficulties describing emotions, as well as imaginative deficits. We suggest that these constructs fall under the umbrella of emotional self-awareness, but represent subtly different aspects of this ability, similar to frameworks for the various aspects of interoception [6].

Previous self-report studies have consistently found lower emotional self-awareness in East Asian populations than in Western populations [7,8,9,10]. These studies used the Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20) [11], a self-report questionnaire assessing alexithymia – difficulty identifying and describing one’s own emotions; it is one of the most commonly used tools to assess emotional self-awareness. The results consistently showed elevated TAS-20 levels in the East-Asian population, compared to those with Western backgrounds. However, it has long been suggested that alexithymia is based on Western cultural norms of emotional expression and communication [12]. As such, there may not be a complete conceptual equivalent of alexithymia across cultures [13], as standards of what represents ‘good’ or ‘normal’ emotional communication varies. Moreover, research suggests that cultural differences in alexithymia scores are more strongly driven by subscales assessing ‘externally oriented thinking’ (i.e., the tendency to focus on external stimuli rather than internal sensations), rather than those reflecting the ability to identify one’s own emotions [8].

In contrast, behavioural studies have provided inconsistent findings, comparing emotional self-awareness in East Asian and Western samples. One study, using the Levels of Emotional Awareness Scale (LEAS) [14], found lower scores in Japanese participants than in American participants [15]. The LEAS is a vignette tool that assesses emotional self-awareness through people’s descriptions of their feelings in response to a variety of hypothetical scenarios. However, it must be noted that this earlier study used different scenarios across the samples and tested the samples under different conditions (i.e., individually vs. in a group). Furthermore, responses describing bodily and somatic sensations are scored lower on the LEAS [14]. It has long been suggested that East Asian cultures and languages are more likely to describe emotions in terms of physiological sensations [13]; as such, differences may be due to linguistic and cultural differences, rather than actual ability. Goetz et al. [16] suggested that the greater dialecticism – the tendency to accept contradiction and change – seen in East Asian cultures leads to greater tolerance of more complex and contradictory emotional states. This results in East Asian participants being more likely to report co-occurring positive and negative states. Furthermore, Grossmann et al. [17] found higher emotional differentiation in Japanese compared to American adults. In this study, American and Japanese participants rated their ten recent emotional experiences in terms of how strongly the experiences elicited nine distinct emotional states. Japanese participants showed stronger differentiation between emotional states of the same valence (i.e., lower intraclass correlations of valence-specific ratings), applying them in different ways to the different scenarios, thus indicating better emotional self-awareness. Moreover, Japanese participants were also more likely to experience mixed emotions (i.e., reporting a higher frequency of co-occurring negative and positive emotions). In short, these behavioural studies suggested lower [15] or higher [16, 17] emotional self-awareness in East Asian compared with American participants.

One explanation for these contradictory findings is that the differences may depend on the measure used, as the measures may differ in task demands and tap into different ‘aspects’ of emotional self-awareness. In the abovementioned studies, lower emotional self-awareness in East Asian samples emerged in self-report analyses, but not in behavioural measures. This may also be due to a greater tendency to modesty in East Asian than in Western cultures [18,19,20], which pertains even in anonymous self-report measures [21]. Such modesty may lead to Japanese samples underreporting their own emotional competence, resulting in elevated TAS-20 scores.

To investigate this issue, we compared the emotional self-awareness of Japanese and United Kingdom (UK) adults using the self-report TAS-20, alongside two behavioural measures, the Emotional Consistency Task (EC-Task) [22] and the Photo Emotion Differentiation Task (PED-Task) [23]. In the EC-Task, participants are presented with pairs of emotional photographs and are instructed to select the one that evokes a stronger emotional experience. The logical consistency of these emotional decisions is examined, generating indices of emotional differentiation. In the PED-Task, participants are presented with individual emotional photographs and are instructed to rate how strongly they feel in terms of 10 emotional states. Correlations between similar emotional states are examined, generating indices of emotional differentiation. The TAS-20 is likely to mirror metacognitive awareness of one’s own emotional abilities. The goal of this study was to examine whether cultural differences in emotional self-awareness are consistent across different measurement tools, in an attempt to evaluate more accurately whether inconsistency in the research literature is related the measurement tool applied. We hypothesised that Japanese participants would show high scores on the TAS-20, but no differences in emotional self-awareness on the EC-Task and PED-Task.

Main text

Methods are presented in Supplementary Material 1. Code and output for main analyses can be seen in Supplementary Material 2.

Findings

All self-report scores were normally distributed. Table 1 presents the average values of key measures, as well as Welch’s t-test comparisons, between cultural groups. Figure 1 shows violin plots of all emotional self-awareness comparisons.

Table 1 Mean (with SD) scores and statistics for main outcomes among Japanese and United Kingdom (UK) samples
Fig. 1
figure 1

Violin plots of emotional self-awareness measures for Japanese and United Kingdom (UK) participants. TAS-20 = Toronto alexithymia scale

Self-reported emotional self-awareness

Welch’s t-test found significantly higher TAS-20 scores in Japanese participants (M = 51.90, SD = 11.17) compared to UK participants (M = 44.44, SD = 11.65), t(62.0) = 2.729, p = .002, d = 0.650. Linear regression analysis was performed to evaluate whether differences remained significant while controlling for gender and age. Only nationality was significant, β = −0.316, p = .011, with neither gender, β = −0.061, p = .608, nor age, β = −0.170, p = .142, reaching significance. These findings suggest that TAS-20 scores were significantly higher in Japanese compared to UK participants, although the R2 was low, with the model only accounting for 8.9% of total variance.

Subscales of the TAS-20 were also compared using Welch’s t-tests. For the Difficulty Identifying Feelings subscale, Japanese participants (M = 16.17, SD = 4.46) scored significantly higher than UK participants (M = 12.84, SD = 4.84), t(63.5) = 2.986, p = .004, d = 0.771, suggesting greater difficulties describing one’s own feelings. A similar effect was found for the Externally Oriented Thinking scale, with Japanese participants (M = 17.83, SD = 4.62) again scoring higher than UK participants (M = 14.23, p = 3.88), t(53.0) = 3.447, p = .001, d = 0.857. No difference emerged for the Difficulty Describing Feelings subscale, t(55.6) = 1.814, p = .070.

Behavioural emotional self-awareness

Behavioural emotional self-awareness was assessed using the EC-Task and PED-Task. In the EC-Task, Welch’s t-test found that Japanese participants (M = 2.00, SD = 0.27) had significantly higher Total Inconsistency scores than UK participants (M = 1.78, SD = 0.33), t(67.7) = 3.105, p = .003, d = 0.730. Linear regression analysis, including performance in the non-emotional control condition, gender, and age as covariates, was conducted to assess the significance of these differences. Nationality significantly predicted Total Inconsistency scores, β = -0.441, p < .001, with being from the UK associated with more consistent emotional decision-making.

In the PED-Task, initial Welch’s t-tests found no differences among cultural groups in terms of positive or negative differentiation, t(64.5) = 1.291 and 1.053, p = .201 and 0.296, d = 0.296 and 0.235, respectively. To confirm this result, a multivariate analyses of covariance (MANCOVA) controlling for age and gender was conducted. MANCOVA was selected so that positive and negative emotion differentiation could be compared in the same analysis. No significant effect for nationality was found, F(2, 67) = 0.975, p = .382. Likewise, no effects were found for either age, F(2, 67) = 2.258, p = .113, or gender, F(2, 67) = 0.179, p = .837.

Discussion

In this study, we examined potential differences in Japanese and UK participants on three measures of emotional self-awareness. In line with previous research [7, 8, 10, 24], TAS-20 scores were significantly higher in Japanese compared to UK samples, indicating lower emotional self-awareness, which was consistent with our hypothesis. Unexpectedly, the EC-Task performance was also lower in Japanese compared to UK samples, indicating lower emotional self-awareness. As expected, the PED-Task showed no clear difference, indicating no cultural difference in the ability to differentiate between qualitatively similar emotional experiences. Notably, while all three measures capture emotional self-awareness, they have different biases and may reflect different aspects of the same ability.

In contrast to our hypothesis, no evident cultural differences were observed in emotional self-awareness between subjective (i.e., TAS-20) and behavioural (i.e., the EC- and PED-Tasks) measures. However, differences were observed between the former two subjective and behavioural tasks and the latter behavioural task, suggesting that differences in these methods could not accounting for cultural differences in emotional self-awareness. We speculate that different levels of metacognitive self-awareness in the measures may underlie these findings. The TAS-20 requires metacognitive self-awareness of emotional ability (i.e., how people rate their own abilities). However, the PED-Task, in which participants rate their online emotional states in response to individually presented emotional photographs, does not require metacognitive self-awareness. Conversely, the EC-Task, in which participants select the photograph evoking stronger emotions from pairs of repetitively presented emotional photographs, uses similar principles to transitive reasoning [22] and may implicitly activate the metacognitive self-awareness to make consistent decisions. Therefore, stronger metacognitive self-awareness of emotional ability in Western compared to East Asian cultures may produce distinct cultural patterns across the tasks. This speculation is consistent with the previous finding that self-monitoring, which is conceptualized as self-observation and self-control [25] and is related to metacognitive awareness [26], was higher in Western (i.e., United States) compared to East Asian (i.e., Japan) cultures [27]. It was proposed that cultures with high individualism focus on the self, rather than others; in contrast, cultures with high collectivism consider the effects on other individuals when deciding how to behave in a particular situation [25]. We speculate that such cultural differences in metacognitive self-awareness may produce divergent cultural patterns in emotional self-awareness, as assessed by self-reported and behavioural measures, among Western and Eastern cultures. Future studies are needed to test these hypotheses empirically.

Limitations

A key limitation of our study was the small sample sizes of the UK (n = 43) and Japanese (n = 29) groups. This constraint raises concerns regarding the generalisability of our findings. A small sample size can lead to several issues, particularly in cross-cultural studies. First, the limited number of participants may not adequately represent the broader populations of Japan and the UK. Cultural diversity within each country means that a small group may not capture the full range of emotional self-awareness present in each culture. Furthermore, small samples limit the power of statistical analyses, making it challenging to draw firm conclusions from the data. Specifically, the lack of significant differences observed in the PED-Task could reflect a lack of power to detect existing differences rather than true similarities between cultures. However, this study was the first to test the same sample with subjective and behavioural tasks and show different patterns across the tasks indicating significant differences in TAS-20 scores and EC-Task performance, but not in PED-Task performance. Thus, our study was a pilot study suggesting that cross-cultural differences in emotional self-awareness may vary with the task used. To validate our findings, future research should investigate larger samples and provide a more reliable and generalisable understanding of the cultural differences in emotional self-awareness.

Data availability

The data that support the findings of this study are available onrequest from the corresponding author.

References

  1. Heyes C. Cognitive gadgets: the cultural evolution of thinking. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 2018.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  2. Tsai JL, Knutson B, Fung HH. Cultural variation in affect valuation. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2006;90:288.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Kashdan TB, Barrett LF, McKnight PE. Unpacking emotion differentiation: transforming unpleasant experience by perceiving distinctions in negativity. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2015;24:10–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Erbas Y, Ceulemans E, Blanke ES, Sels L, Fischer A, Kuppens P. Emotion differentiation dissected: Between-category, within-category, and integral emotion differentiation, and their relation to well-being. Cogn Emo. 2019;33:258–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Sifneos PE. The prevalence of ‘alexithymic’ characteristics in psychosomatic patients. Psychother Psychosom. 1973;22:255–62.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Garfinkel SN, Seth AK, Barrett AB, Suzuki K, Critchley HD. Knowing your own heart: distinguishing interoceptive accuracy from interoceptive awareness. Biol Psychol. 2015;104:65–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Dere J, Falk CF, Ryder AG. Unpacking cultural differences in alexithymia: the role of cultural values among Euro-Canadian and chinese-canadian students. J Cross-Cult Psychol. 2012;43:1297–312.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Dere J, Tang Q, Zhu X, Cai L, Yao S, Ryder AG. The cultural shaping of alexithymia: values and externally oriented thinking in a Chinese clinical sample. Compr Psychiatry. 2013;54:362–68.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Dion KL. Ethnolinguistic correlates of alexithymia: toward a cultural perspective. J Psychosom Res. 1996;41:531–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Le HN, Berenbaum H, Raghavan C. Culture and alexithymia: Mean levels, correlates and the role of parental Socialization of emotions. Emotion. 2002;2:341–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Bagby RM, Parker JDA, Taylor GJ. The twenty-item Toronto Alexithymia scale—I. item selection and cross-validation of the factor structure. J Psychosom Res. 1994;38:23–32.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Kirmayer LJ. Languages of suffering healing: Alexithymia as a social and cultural process. Transcult Psychiatric Res Rev. 1987;24:119–36.

    Google Scholar 

  13. Zhou P, Critchley H, Garfinkel S, Gao Y. The conceptualization of emotions across cultures: a model based on interoceptive neuroscience. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2021;125:314–27.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Lane RD, Quinlan DM, Schwartz GE, Walker PA, Zeitlin SB. The levels of emotional awareness scale: a cognitive-developmental measure of emotion. J Pers Assess. 1990;55:124–34.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Igarashi T, Komaki G, Lane RD, Moriguchi Y, Nishimura H, Arakawa H, Gondo M, Terasawa Y, Sullivan CV, Maeda M. The reliability and validity of the Japanese version of the levels of emotional awareness scale (LEAS-J). Biopsychosoc Med. 2011;5:2.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Goetz JL, Spencer-Rodgers J, Peng K. Dialectical emotions: how cultural epistemologies influence the experience and regulation of emotional complexity. In: Sorrentino RM, Yamaguchi S, editors. Handbook of motivation and cognition across cultures. Cambridge, MA: Academic Press; 2008;517–39.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  17. Grossmann I, Huynh AC, Ellsworth PC. Emotional complexity: clarifying definitions and cultural correlates. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2016;111:895.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Brown R. Censure avoidance and self-esteem in Japan. J Soc Psychol. 2008;148:653–66.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Hashimoto H, Li Y, Yamagishi T. Beliefs and preferences in cultural agents and cultural game players. Asian J Soc Psychol. 2011;14:140–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Kitayama S, Markus HR, Matsumoto H, Norasakkunkit V. Individual and collective processes in the construction of the self: self-enhancement in the United States and self-criticism in Japan. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1997;72:1245.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Brown R. The effect of anonymity on self-evaluations in Japanese college students. Inf Commun Soc. 2006;34:11–8.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Huggins CF, Cameron IM, Williams JHG. Autistic traits predict underestimation of emotional abilities. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2021;150:930–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Erbas Y, Ceulemans E, Lee Pe M, Koval P, Kuppens P. Negative emotion differentiation: its personality and well-being correlates and a comparison of different assessment methods. Cogn Emo. 2014;28:1196–213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Lo C. Cultural values and alexithymia. SAGE Open. 2014;4:2158244014555117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Syner M. Self-monitoring of expressive behavior. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1974;12:526–37.

    Google Scholar 

  26. Petty RE, Briñol P, Tormala ZL, Wegener DT. The role of meta-cognition in social judgment. In: Kruglanski AK, Higgins ET, editors. Social psychology: handbook of basic principles. New York: Guilford Press; 2007;254–84.

    Google Scholar 

  27. Gudykunst WB, Yang SM, Nishida T. Cultural differences in self-consciousness and self-monitoring. Commun Res. 1987;14:7–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to particularly thank Mr. Usami for testing the participants and his tireless assistance with recruitment, as well as the staff at the Kokoro Research Centre for testing the translation and providing feedback.

Funding

This work was supported by the Japanese Society for the Promotion of Science (JSPS), under the Summer Programme, Grant SP19103.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

CFH contributed to study design and analysis, and provided initial draft of manuscript. JHGW contributed to study design, and provided feedback on early draft of manuscript. SW contributed to study design, recruitment, and revised the manuscript for publication.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Wataru Sato.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Ethics Committee of the Unit for Advanced Studies of the Human Mind, Kyoto University (30-P-7). Written informed consent was obtained from all participants after the experimental procedure was fully explained.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary Material 1

Supplementary Material 2

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Huggins, C.F., Williams, J.H.G. & Sato, W. Cross-cultural differences in self-reported and behavioural emotional self-awareness between Japan and the UK. BMC Res Notes 16, 380 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13104-023-06660-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13104-023-06660-0

Keywords