Skip to main content

Limited training and transfer effects in older and young adults who participated in 12 sessions of process-based working memory training. A three-armed pretest-posttest design study

Abstract

Objective

Numerous studies confirm the effectiveness of cognitive training in older adults. However, there is limited evidence of the transfer occurrence. The part of the study presented here tested the effect of 12 process-based working memory training sessions on the performance of the trained task (training effect) and other cognitive tasks (transfer effect). A pretest-posttest study design with one experimental group and two control (passive and active) groups. The sample comprised three groups of older adults: experimental (n = 25), passive control (n = 22), active control (n = 7), and young adults: experimental (n = 25), passive control (n = 25), and active control (n = 12). The study was registered after completion with a ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT06235840 on 31 January 2024.

Results

Under the influence of training, the performance of the trained task improved significantly, but only in young adults. Transfer of WM training effects was not revealed. Among young adults, a testing effect was observed for the indicator of attentional focus and psychomotor speed. Moreover, the obtained results suggest the transfer from practice in multi-domain training, implemented in the active control group, to tasks that require the use of fluid intelligence. However, this finding should be interpreted with great caution due to the small size of active control groups.

Peer Review reports

Introduction

Numerous studies have shown that cognitive training is effective among cognitively healthy older individuals. At the same time, small to moderate effects are usually obtained and not for all indicators of cognitive functioning considered [1,2,3,4,5,6]. One of the issues crucial for the field of study on cognitive training effectiveness is the problem of cognitive transfer. The generalization of skills across similar cognitive domains or different tasks involving the same domain is known as near transfer. In contrast, the generalization across domains that are very weakly related (or even unrelated) indicates far transfer [7,8,9]. In this context, researchers are particularly interested in the effects of process-based training, which is based on the assumption that cognitive functions can be improved by repeatedly exercising the core processes, like working memory (WM), attentional control, or executive functions (EF), underlying them [8,9,10,11]. So far, the effectiveness of this type of training has been tested in numerous studies. However, even the results of meta-analyses have been inconclusive and contentious [12,13,14,15]. Concerning the age group of older adults, meta-analyses on the transfer of the effects of WM training have consistently shown that transfer, though possible, is limited [10, 16], and the following pattern would appear to be reproducible: (a) large training effect (improvement in the performance of trained tasks); (b) small or no near transfer effect; (c) no effect or eventually very little effect of long-distance transfer [17, 18].

In a study in which we tested the effect of WM training on electrophysiological brain activity (National Science Centre, Poland, grant no 2017/25/B/HS6/00360), we also examined the magnitude of improvement in the trained task (training effect) and changes under the influence of training in the performance of non-trained cognitive tasks with varying degrees of cognitive similarity to the trained task (near and far transfer effects). This part of the results is presented in this article. The full study was registered after completion with a ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT06235840 on 31 January 2024. Results on brain activity accompanying WM involvement in younger and older adults collected in a pretest measurement have been published [19], and a report on the impact of WM training on brain activity is in preparation.

Method

A pretest-posttest study design was applied with one experimental group and two control (passive and active control) groups. We defined the active control group as receiving an alternative intervention comparable to the experimental group but not involving the WM training of interest [20, 21]. In the study, young adults were included in addition to older participants to allow comparisons of training effectiveness according to age.

Selection for the sample was voluntary. Participants registered in response to advertisements distributed online and on public transport in Bydgoszcz (Poland). At the same time, participants had to meet the following inclusion criteria: (1) age: 60–75 years old or 20–35 years old, where the 60–75 age group is classified as early late-adulthood, and the 20–35 group as early adulthood [22]; (2) no mental illness and neurological disorders (verified by structured interview and the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview – M.I.N.I. 7.0 [23]; (3) normal or corrected-to-normal vision; (4) no dementia symptoms (verified in older adults with the Polish version of the Mini-Mental State Examination, MMSE, which was purchased from the Psychological Testing Laboratory of the Polish Psychological Association, which holds the rights to the MMSE on the Polish market.) [24]; (5) signing an informed consent to participate in the study (after familiarizing oneself with the aim of the study and the conditions of participation, as well as having received satisfactory answers to all questions). The study sample comprised 116 participants including (a) older adults, divided into three research groups: experimental (E; n = 25; 22 women; age: M = 66.40, SD = 3.55), passive control (PC; n = 22; 16 women; age: M = 66.77, SD = 4.42), active control (AC; n = 7; 6 women; age: M = 65.29, SD = 3.35), and (b) young adults, also divided into three research groups experimental (n = 25; 15 women; age: M = 26.48, SD = 5.13), passive control (n = 25; 15 women; age: M = 25.48, SD = 4.81), active control (n = 12; 11 women; age: M = 23.92, SD = 5.23).

In the experimental group, participants took part in process-based adaptive training using an n-back task. In the active control group, non-adaptive multi-domain training was introduced. In both experimental and active control groups, participants were involved in 12 sessions, three in each of the four weeks. Each session lasted approximately 45 min. The passive control group was the no-contact group.

The n-back task involves WM in terms of its content updating [25, 26]. This task is based on the continuous presentation of items (letters in this study) that appear and disappear one by one. During each presentation, the participant must judge whether the currently displayed item matches the item presented ‘n’ trials earlier [27]. The n-back task used in the current study was computerized and was programmed in PsychoPy software [28]. All participants started their training from the 1-back level. The training was adaptive, meaning participants progressed to higher (or dropped to lower) difficulty levels according to the performance accuracy achieved. The increasing/decreasing difficulty was based on a rise/fall in the ‘n’ parameter.

In the active control group, non-adaptive multi-domain computerized training was introduced, which included tasks involving visual-spatial functions, visual and verbal memory, analogical reasoning on visual material, deductive reasoning on verbal material, and calculia.

The assignment to the research groups was not random and was based on an n-back task and Raven’s Standard Progressive Matrices performance because the aim was to equalize the cognitive functioning of individual research groups (within age groups).

A series of measures of cognitive functioning were used in the pretest and posttest: (a) the n-back task at three levels of difficulty, i.e. 1-back, 2-back, and 3-back, in which the sensitivity index (d’) was used as the indicator of task performance; d’ was calculated with the formula for yes/no tasks: d’ = z(H) – z(FA), where H and FA are the Hits and False Alarm rates, respectively, and z(H) and z(FA) are the z-transformations [29]; (b) computerized OperationSpan Task (OSPAN) [30]– to assess WM capacity; (c) two subtests from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale – WAIS-R(PL) [31]: the Digit Span Subtest (Dig-Span) to measure short-term memory and WM capacity, and The Digit-Symbol Coding Subtest (Dig-Symb) to measure attentional focus and psychomotor speed; (d) Raven’s Standard Progressive Matrices in Polish standardization (TMS) [32] to measure fluid intelligence, the raw score was used as the performance indicator, 50% of the sample performed the classic version in the pretest and the parallel version in the posttest, while 50% did the opposite.

Results

To assess the effect of training on the performance of the trained task (n-back), a mixed-design ANOVA was conducted, with two within-person factors, i.e., repeated measurement (pretest-posttest) and the n-back task difficulty level, and two between-person factors: research group and age group (Fig. 1). As expected, the effect of the age group proved to be significant (F = 73.51; p < .001; η2p = 0.42), with poorer performance in the older adult group. No significant interaction effect of age group and research group was observed (F = 0.48; p = .623; η2p = 0.01). Also in line with expectations, significant was the effect of the n-back task difficulty level (F = 222.58; p < .001; η2p = 0.69), with decreasing levels of performance at increasing difficulty. At the same time, there was a significant interaction effect of the task difficulty with the age group (F = 5.50; p = .005; η2p = 0.05) but not with the research group (F = 1.43; p = .238; η2p = 0.03). The main effect of repeated measurement was found to be statistically significant (F = 38.09; p < .001; η2p = 0.28), as well as the interaction of repeated measurement with the research group (F = 6.65; p = .002; η2p = 0.12) but not with the age group (F = 0.16; p = .694; η2p = 0.002). The interaction effect of the three factors, i.e., repeated measurement, age group, and research group, was not statistically significant (F = 1.39; p = .191; η2p = 0.03). However, the interaction effect of the four factors, i.e., the three mentioned above plus the n-back task difficulty level, did (F = 2.60; p = .037; η2p = 0.05).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Performance of the n-back task (d’) at three difficulty levels in the initial and final measurements in the experimental, passive control, and active control groups of older and young adults

In further analyses, a series of ANOVA was performed for each cognitive measure considered in the study, including three levels of the n-back task. For the results collected in the pretest and posttest (separately), the main effects of age group, research group, and effect of their interaction were analysed. Furthermore, the effect of repeated measurement (pretest vs. posttest) and its interaction with age group, research group, and both of those factors were tested. Each ANOVA was supplemented by post hoc analysis using the Tukey-Kramer test. The results of the series of ANOVA are presented in Table 1, while the means and standard deviations of each cognitive measure at the pretest and posttest for each age and research group are presented in Table 2. The results of the post hoc analysis are reported in the text, when appropriate, as a supplement to the ANOVA results.

Table 1 Mixed-design ANOVA results for pretest, posttest, and repeated measurement for trained and not-trained cognitive tasks performance
Table 2 The means and standard deviations of the scores of the particular cognitive measures in each research group (experimental, passive control, and active control) and age group (older and young adults)

According to the results, older adults show significantly worse cognitive performance than young adults in the pretest and posttest for all cognitive measures, except for the Digit Span Forward task, which involves short-term memory.

At the pretest, the research groups do not differ significantly in cognitive functioning. Only in the 3-back task did the study group effect prove to be statistically significant, but post hoc analysis revealed no significant differences between research groups in either older (E vs. PC p = 1.00; E vs. AC p = .223; PC vs. AC p = .296) or young adults (E vs. PC p = .912; E vs. AC p = .811; PC vs. AC p = .993).

Significant changes between the initial and final measurements were observed on several cognitive measures (Table 1). At the same time, a significant interaction effect of repeated measurement with the research group was revealed only for the trained 3-back task (where, in addition, the interaction effect of three factors: repeated measurement, research group, and age group was significant) and for the untrained TMS test. Moreover, in the case of the 3-back task, the effects of the research group and the interaction of the research group and age were significant in the posttest, which was not observed for the TMS test. Post hoc analysis revealed that significant improvements in performance on the 3-back task occurred only in the experimental group of young adults (p < .001), not in the other research groups of young adults (PC p = 1.00; AC p = .446) and older adults (E p = .136; PC p = 1.00; AC p = .998). As for differences between the research groups in the posttest, among young adults, the experimental group performed significantly higher than the passive control (p < .001) and active control (p < .001) groups, which, in turn, did not differ from each other (p = .912). This result was not replicated in older adults, where there was no significant difference between the research groups in the posttest (E vs. PC p = .467; E vs. AC p = .188; PC vs. AC p = .787). Therefore, based on the results obtained, it can be concluded that the performance of the 3-back task improved under the influence of training, but only in young adults. In contrast, for the TMS test, a significant improvement was revealed by post hoc analysis only in the active control group of older adults (p = .023), not in the other research groups of older adults (E p = 1.00; PC p = .518), and any research group of young participants, although in the passive control group of young adults the difference reached the level of statistical trend (E p = .915; PC p = 1.00; AC p = .069). Thus, in the case of the TMS test, we have a rationale for inferring a near transfer from multi-domain training (covering tasks involving analogical reasoning) to the performance of a test measuring fluid intelligence, primarily in older adults.

In contrast, for the 1-back, 2-back, and Digit Span Backward tasks, only the effect of repeated measurement was significant, with no interaction with the study group. Such a result reflects the testing effect (test-enhanced learning) rather than the impact of training. At the same time, while the research group effect was statistically significant in the 2-back task in the posttest, the post hoc analysis did not show statistically significant differences between the research groups in older adults (E vs. PC p = .583; E vs. AC p = .796; PC vs. AC p = .998) or young adults (E vs. PC p = .442; E vs. AC p = .241; PC vs. AC p = .944). In parallel, according to the results of the post hoc analysis, a significant improvement in the performance of the 2-back task occurred only in the experimental group of young participants (p = .018), not in the other research groups of young adults (PC p = .362; AC p = .970) nor older adults (E p = .400; PC p = 1.00; AC p = .847). In the 1-back task, on the other hand, there was no significant change in the young participants (E p = .614; PC p = .994; AC p = .972), while the older ones showed improvement at the level of statistical trend but only in the active control group (E p = .260; PC p = 1.00; AC p = .059).

Furthermore, for the Digit Symbol task, significant effects of repeated measurement and the interaction of repeated measurement with age group were revealed, which may suggest the presence of an age-dependent testing effect. This interpretation is supported by the results of the post hoc analysis, which disclose a significant improvement in performance on this task in each of the research groups of young adults (E p < .001; PC p < .001; AC p = .044) but in none of the older adult groups (E p = .327; PC p = .960; AC p = 1.00).

Finally, for the OSPAN task, the interaction effect of repeated measurement with age group was found without the significance of the main effect of repeated measurement. However, post hoc analyses indicated a significant improvement in performance on this task only in the passive control group of older adults (p = .035) but not in the other research groups of older adults (E p = .897; AC p = 1.00) or young adults (E p = .921; PC p = 1.00; AC p = 1.00). This result eludes interpretation in the light of the assumptions made based on the existing knowledge and seems to bear the mark of coincidence.

Discussion

The aim of the part of the study presented here was to test whether WM training based on an n-back task leads to improved performance on trained tasks and the transfer to other cognitive tasks among older adults and young adults.

The results indicate that the training positively affected the performance of the trained task, but only among young adults and at higher levels of task difficulty. The differences in the older adults’ experimental group were too small to prove significant in post hoc analyses. This finding is inconsistent with the conclusions of the meta-analyses presented in the introduction. However, the training implemented in the current study was relatively short, which may have influenced its effects. On the other hand, it is possible that process-based training involving WM has limited effectiveness among older adults.

Going further, we do not observe the phenomenon of the near and far transfer of practice from the n-back task to other cognitive tasks. The lack of transfer among older adults could be explained by the limited effects of the training itself. Still, transfer also did not occur in young adults, in whom training significantly improved performance on the trained n-back task. However, in young adults, there was an apparent testing effect (test-enhanced learning) for performance on a task requiring attentional focus and psychomotor speed.

Interesting appear to be the results regarding changes in performance on a test measuring fluid intelligence. This is because they suggest the transfer from practice in multi-domain training, implemented in the active control group, to tasks that require the use of fluid intelligence. Such results could be explained by the use, as a part of multi-domain training, tasks that enforce analogical reasoning and rule recognition, which are also needed when performing TMS test. Thus, it could be argued that a phenomenon of near transfer is revealed for training treated as an active control. This result, however, should be interpreted with great caution due to the small size of active control groups, especially among older adults. At the same time, it can be treated as a contribution to further research.

Limitations

  • The main limitation of the study is the unequal size of the research groups, or more precisely, the small size of the active control groups. This significantly limits the ability to conclude based on the effects obtained for these groups.

  • The research and age groups are not gender balanced. Moreover, active control groups appear younger than the other two research groups (within each age group), but there are no statistically significant differences in age between the three research groups among older adults (F = 0.38; p = .683; η2p = 0.02) and young adults (F = 1.07; p = .351; η2p = 0.04).

  • The baseline levels of performance of the cognitive tasks used are not perfectly equal in individual research groups (within age groups). However, as reported above, there were no statistically significant differences in the performance of tasks that were the criteria for group assignment.

Data availability

Data is provided within the manuscript or supplementary information files.The study was registered after completion of the study with a ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT06235840 on 31 January 2024.

Abbreviations

AC:

Active control group

d’:

Sensitivity index

Dig-Span:

The Digit Span Subtest from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale

Sig-Symb:

The Digit-Symbol Coding Subtest from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale

E:

Experimental group

EFL:

Executive functions

MANOVA:

Multivariate analysis of variance

M.I.N.I. 7.0:

The Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview

MMSE:

The Mini-Mental State Examination

OSPAN:

The Operation Span task

PC:

Passive control group

T-K:

The Tukey-Kramer post hoc test

TMS:

Raven’s Standard Progressive Matrices in Polish Standardization

WAIS-R(PL):

The Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale

WM:

Working memory

References

  1. Chiu HL, Chu H, Tsai JC, Liu D, Chen YR, Yang HL et al. The effect of cognitive-based training for the healthy older people: a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. PLoS ONE. 2017;12(5).

  2. Lampit A, Hallock H, Valenzuela M. Computerized cognitive training in cognitively healthy older adults: a systematic review and Meta-analysis of Effect modifiers. PLoS Med. 2014;11(11).

  3. Yun S, Ryu S. The effects of cognitive-based interventions in older adults: a systematic review and Meta-analysis. Iran J Public Health. 2022;51(1):1–11.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Lee YY, Wu CY, Teng CH, Hsu WC, Chang KC, Chen P. Evolving methods to combine cognitive and physical training for individuals with mild cognitive impairment: study protocol for a randomized controlled study. Trials. 2016;17(1):1–11.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  5. Bonnechère B, Langley C, Sahakian BJ. The use of commercial computerised cognitive games in older adults: a meta-analysis. Sci Rep. 2020;10(1):1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Tetlow AM, Edwards JD. Systematic Literature Review and Meta-analysis of commercially available computerized cognitive training among older adults. J Cogn Enhanc. 2017;1(4):559–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Barnett SM, Ceci SJ. When and where do we apply what we learn? A taxonomy for far transfer. Psychol Bull. 2002;128(4):612–37.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Taatgen NA. The nature and transfer of cognitive skills. Psychol Rev. 2013;120(3):439–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Zając-Lamparska L, Trempała J. Effects of working memory and attentional control training and their transfer onto fluid intelligence in early and late adulthood. Heal Psychol Rep. 2016;4(1):41–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Karbach J, Verhaeghen P. Making Working Memory Work: a Meta-analysis of executive-control and working memory training in older adults. Psychol Sci. 2014;25(11):2027–37.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Stine-Morrow EAL, Basak C. Cognitive interventions. Handbook of the psychology of aging. Elsevier; 2011. pp. 153–71.

  12. Au J, Sheehan E, Tsai N, Duncan GJ, Buschkuehl M, Jaeggi SM. Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory: a meta-analysis. Psychon Bull Rev. 2015;22(2):366–77.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Au J, Buschkuehl M, Duncan GJ, Jaeggi SM. There is no convincing evidence that working memory training is NOT effective: a reply to Melby-Lervåg and Hulme (2015). Psychon Bull Rev. 2016;23(1):331–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Melby-Lervåg M, Hulme C. Is working memory training effective? A meta-analytic review. Dev Psychol. 2013;49(2):270–91.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Melby-Lervåg M, Hulme C. There is no convincing evidence that working memory training is effective: a reply to au et al. (2014) and Karbach and Verhaeghen (2014). Psychon Bull Rev. 2016;23(1):324–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Soveri A, Antfolk J, Karlsson L, Salo B, Laine M. Working memory training revisited: a multi-level meta-analysis of n-back training studies. Psychon Bull Rev. 2017;24(4):1077–96.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Sala G, Aksayli ND, Tatlidil KS, Gondo Y, Gobet F. Working memory training does not enhance older adults’ cognitive skills: a comprehensive meta-analysis. Intelligence. 2019;77(June):101386.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Teixeira-Santos AC, Moreira CS, Magalhães R, Magalhães C, Pereira DR, Leite J, et al. Reviewing working memory training gains in healthy older adults: a meta-analytic review of transfer for cognitive outcomes. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2019;103:163–77.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Zając-Lamparska L, Zabielska-Mendyk E, Zapała D, Augustynowicz P. Compensatory brain activity pattern is not present in older adults during the n-back task performance-findings based on EEG frequency analysis. Front Psychol. 2024;15:1371035.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Temple R, Ellenberg SS. Placebo-controlled trials and active-control trials in the evaluation of new treatments. Part 1: ethical and scientific issues. Ann Intern Med. 2000;133(6):455–63.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Au J, Gibson BC, Bunarjo K, Buschkuehl M, Jaeggi SM. Quantifying the difference between active and Passive Control Groups in Cognitive interventions using two Meta-Analytical approaches. J Cogn Enhanc. 2020;4(2):192–210.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Sugarman L. Life-span development: frameworks, accounts and strategies. Life-span development: Frameworks, accounts and strategies. Taylor and Francis; 2004. pp. 1–256.

  23. Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Amorim P, Janavs J, Weiller E, et al. The mini-international neuropsychiatric interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J Clin Psychiatry. 1998;59(SUPPL 20):22–33.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Stańczak J. MMSE: Polska normalizacja [MMSE: Polish normalization]. Warszawa: Pracownia Testów Psychologicznych; 2010. pp. 1–42.

    Google Scholar 

  25. Jaeggi SM, Buschkuehl M, Jonides J, Perrig WJ. Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008;105(19):6829–33.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Schmiedek F, Lövdén M, Lindenberger U. A task is a task: putting complex span, n-back, and other working memory indicators in psychometric context. Front Psychol. 2014;5(DEC):1–8.

    Google Scholar 

  27. Zając-Lamparska L. Relationship between Working Memory, Fluid Intelligence and Age based on the results of mediation and moderation analyses. Ann Psychol. 2021;24(3):279–305.

    Google Scholar 

  28. Peirce JW. Generating stimuli for neuroscience using PsychoPy. Front Neuroinform. 2009;2(10):1–8.

    Google Scholar 

  29. Stanislaw H, Todorov N. Calculation of signal detection theory measures. Behav Res Methods Instruments Comput. 1999;3(I):137–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Turner ML, Engle RW. Is working memory capacity task dependent? J Mem Lang. 1989;28(2):127–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Brzeziński J, Hornowska E, Jaworowska A, Machowski A, Zakrzewska M. Skala Inteligencji Wechslera Dla Dorosłych - Wersja Zrewidowana - Renormalizacja WAIS-R(PL) [Wechsler Intelligence Scale for adults - revised version - renormalization WAIS-R(PL)]. Warszawa: Pracownia Testów Psychologicznych; 2011.

    Google Scholar 

  32. Jaworowska A, Szustrowa T. Test Matryc Ravena w wersji standard TMS. Formy: Klasyczna, Równoległa, Plus: polskie standaryzacje. [Raven’s Matrix Test in the Standard version: TMS. Forms: classical, parallel, plus: Polish standardizations]. 2nd ed. Warszawa: Pracownia Testów Psychologicznych; 2007. pp. 1–137.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The author would like to thank the scholarship holders: Maciej Bącała and Martyna Krawczyk, and interns: Wiktoria Fronskiewicz, Weronika Groszek, Monika Oziemblewska, and Agnieszka Schmeichel, who were involved in acquiring data and conducting training sessions, as well as to scholarship holder Łukasz Warchoł for programming n-back tasks in the PsychoPy environment.

Funding

This study was funded by the National Science Centre, Poland (Grant No. 2017/25/B/HS6/00360). The funding body did not play any role in the design of the study and collection, analysis, and interpretation of data and in writing the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

LZL: conception and design of the study; obtaining funds for the study; data acquisition; organizing the database; statistical analysis; interpretation of the results of statistical analysis; writing the manuscript; reading and approving the submitted version.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ludmiła Zając-Lamparska.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. The study protocol and the informed consent form were approved on 27/02/2018 by the Bioethics Committee of the Nicolaus Copernicus University in Toruń functioning at Collegium Medicum in Bydgoszcz (ref. 90/2018), which acts according to the regulation of the Minister of Health and Welfare of May 11. 1999 on detailed rules for the creation, financing, and functioning of bioethics committees (Dz. U. [Journal of Laws] No 47, item 480). The study was registered after completion with a ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT06235840 on 31 January 2024. Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study. All participants gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. This article does not contain any studies with animals performed by any of the authors.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary Material 1

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Zając-Lamparska, L. Limited training and transfer effects in older and young adults who participated in 12 sessions of process-based working memory training. A three-armed pretest-posttest design study. BMC Res Notes 17, 181 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13104-024-06844-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13104-024-06844-2

Keywords