Skip to main content

Interoceptive awareness in a Southeastern US college sample: validation of the multidimensional assessment of interoceptive awareness – version 2

Abstract

Objective

The Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness, version 2 (MAIA-2) is a commonly utilized self-report instrument to assess individuals’ ability to perceive bodily sensations. The MAIA-2 has displayed variable psychometric properties across samples. Thus, we examine the psychometric properties of the MAIA-2 in a Southeastern United States college sample.

Participants

Our studies consisted of 710 (study 1) and 66 (study 2) college students.

Methods

Study 1 used a cross-sectional research design where we investigated the factor structure, and measurement invariance (e.g., measured similarly across genders). Study 2 examined the test-retest reliability across a three-week period.

Results

The MAIA-2 displayed adequate to good internal consistencies and factor loadings, strict invariance, and questionable temporal stability.

Conclusion

The MAIA-2 demonstrates adequate psychometric properties in this college sample that were similar to the original MAIA sample characteristics. Contextual and cultural factors may influence the subjective experience of interpreting bodily sensations.

Peer Review reports

Introduction

University counseling centers (UCC) have hired more therapists and emphasized outreach programming to accommodate the increased demand for services and to meet the continued rise of mental health concerns for college students [1,2,3]. Mindfulness and body-based psychotherapies, which have demonstrated positive results for depression, anxiety, and self-compassion [4,5,6], provide one such way UCCs meet this need. The effectiveness of these interventions may be assessed through changes in interoceptive awareness [7, 8].

Interoceptive awareness is characterized by the self-reported ability to consciously perceive and judge internal bodily sensations and is associated with various mental health conditions [9,10,11,12,13]. For example, college students’ interoceptive awareness was related to emotional eating [14, 15], body appreciation, intuitive eating [16], and anxiety [17]. The Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness Version 2 (MAIA-2) is a 37-item, eight-dimension measure of self-reported interoceptive awareness [13], is the second most used measure of interoceptive awareness [18], and has demonstrated varying psychometric properties across different samples [12, 13, 18,19,20,21,22,23,24,25,26,27,28,29]. The MAIA-2’s constructs capture a person’s awareness, trust, and tendency to listen to bodily sensations, a tendency to avoid or be distressed with bodily discomfort, attention regulation toward bodily sensations, awareness of physiological sensations with emotional experiences, and ability to regulate via bodily sensation [12].

Mehling and colleagues’ original sample consisted of largely white female participants, over half completed graduate education, and all engaged in some form of somatic practice (e.g., yoga) [12]. The MAIA-2 sample included participants visiting the Science Museum of London, UK, who were 47% female and 60% native English speakers, but no information about fluency or acculturation for non-native speakers was collected [13]. The MAIA’s extensive use has led to validations in various populations. Researchers from six countries (Portugal [24], Japan [25, 26], Chile [27], Italy [23], Lithuania [22], and Columbia [28]) evaluated the psychometric properties of the MAIA in different college samples. These studies demonstrated factor structures that included the observation of eight [23, 27], seven [24], and six dimensions [22, 25, 26], with questionable to acceptable model fit and internal consistency values (e.g., α ranging from 0.40 [27] to 0.87 [24,25,26]). A full review of these validations is beyond the scope of this report and for further interest see Todd et al. [29] and the MAIA website [30].

There is a need to validate the MAIA-2 in a US college population, given that (1) college students’ mental health is an increasing concern [1,2,3], (2) difficulties with interoceptive awareness are associated with various mental health conditions [14,15,16,17], (3) college student samples demonstrated different factor structures [23,24,25,26,27,28], (4) the MAIA-2 is commonly used [18], and (5) to answer the call for replication studies [31]. Therefore, we aim to examine the psychometric properties of the MAIA-2 in a United States college sample by examining the internal validity, factor structure, and test-retest reliability.

Main text

Study 1

Methods

Our university’s Institutional Review Board (STUDY00006902) reviewed and approved the project measures and procedures. The study included 710 undergraduate and graduate students from the University of Georgia. Participants were majority white (63.2%) and female (69.6%), with a mean age of 23.77 years (SD = 7.29). The participants provided their informed consent and then completed the MAIA-2, demographics, and other measures beyond the scope of this study. Complete demographic information appears in supplementary material Table 1.

Data analysis

All analyses were performed with R (version 4.2.2) [32]. Descriptive data, distribution skewness, and kurtosis were obtained, and missing items were assessed using Little’s Missing at Random (MCAR) [33] and a maximum likelihood procedure in the MICE package [34]. The data were evaluated for internal consistency as measured by Cronbach’s alpha [35], and McDonald’s Omega [36], using the Coefficient alpha package [37], where > 0.70 is acceptable, > 0.8 is good, and 0.9 > is excellent [38].

We assessed the fit of the original eight-factor measurement model [12, 13] using confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) with the maximum likelihood estimation with robust standard errors and a Satorra-Bentler scaled test statistic. Goodness-of-fit of the model was evaluated using the Comparative Fit Index (CFI) and Tucker Lewis Index (TLI) (good fit \(\:\ge\:\) 0.95), Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA; good fit \(\:\le\:\:\)0.05; adequate fit \(\:\le\:\) 0.08), and Standardized Root Mean Square Residual (SRMR; acceptable fit \(\:\le\:\) 0.08) [39, 40]. Modification indices were examined to assess for improved model fit. All factor calculations were completed using the Lavaan package in R [41].

Thirteen participants identified as gender-diverse, with two missing gender identities. Due to this, the invariance testing was conducted on binary gender identities (n = 695; males [n = 494]; females [n = 201]) to ensure adequate numbers in each group, while all other analyses included the full sample. We examined multiple nested models in a forward approach, using (1) configural invariance (i.e., assessing if factor structure is equal across groups), (2) metric invariance (i.e., assessing if factor loadings are equal across groups), and (3) scalar invariance (i.e., assessing if item intercepts are equal across groups) [40, 42, 43]. Invariance was assessed using the cutoff criteria of ΔCFI < 0.01 and ΔRMSEA < 0.15 [40, 42, 43].

Results

The sample had a total of 17 (0.06%) missing items. Little’s MCAR test suggested that the data were missing at random; thus, the 17 missing items were estimated using the MICE [34] package in R.

Internal consistency

The results suggested good values for the internal consistencies of the subscale items for ND, NW, AR, SR, BL, and TR and adequate internal consistency values for the NT and EA subscales (see Table 1). Students endorsed a tendency to use distraction as a coping strategy and trust their bodies, as indicated by the percentile ranges.

Table 1 Results of the Confirmatory Factor Analysis and Subscales means, SD, and internal consistency
Confirmatory factor analysis

The results show acceptable measures of model fit using the RMSEA (0.60 [0.057, 0.063]) and the SRMR (0.067) and questionable measures of fit indices for CFI (0.855), TLI (0.839), and x2 = 1812.799, df = 601, p < 0.001. Standardized factor loadings indicate a meaningful level of item contribution to each factor (0.51–0.92). Table 2 in the supplementary material includes all factor loadings for the MAIA-2 measurement model.

We examined modification indices (MI) and included seven covariance terms across 14 items to improve the model fit. The results of the MAIA-2 with MI exhibited good measures of fit indices for RMSEA (0.046 [0.043, 0.049]) and SRMR (0.063) and improved measures of fit indices for CFI (0.917), TLI (0.906), and x2 = 1304.558, df = 594, p < 0.001. The results suggest adequate standardized factor loadings (0.41–0.93). Table 1 displays all factor loadings with MI included.

Measurement invariance

Table 2 shows the changes in chi-square, CFI, and RMSEA as model constraints were added. The sample met the recommended ΔCFI < 0.01 and ΔRMSEA < 0.15 and supported equal factor structure (configural), item loadings (metric), and intercepts (scalar) across groups. Thus, the model fit for the MAIA-2 demonstrated scalar invariance between men and women in our sample.

Table 2 Measurement invariance of the MAIA-2 for women (n = 494) and men (n = 201)

Discussion

The results from Study 1 suggested that the MAIA-2 has generally good psychometric properties, with some qualifications. First, the items appeared internally consistent with how participants understood and responded, which has varied in college sample validation studies [23:29]. Unlike previous studies [12, 13, 29], the NW and ND items demonstrated consistent participant responses. Second, the items displayed adequate coverage of multiple dimensions of interoceptive awareness, with questionable- to good-fit for the measurement model and comparable with the original studies [12, 13]. Lastly, the results demonstrated invariance between men’s and women’s responses, which means that the factor analytic results do not differ according to the student’s gender.

Study 2

Materials

We examined the test-retest reliability of the MAIA-2 using a three-week test-retest interval. The University of Georgia’s IRB approved the research (PROJECT00005184). The material consisted of a printed, hard-copy packet that contained the MAIA-2 and a demographic form. The participants were recruited through a university research pool, partially fulfilled the students’ academic research requirements, and were required to schedule both administration dates. Sixty-six participants completed the research in groups ranging from four to twenty people. At the first session, the participants were provided an overview of the study and written informed consent, which was signed before administering the measures. In the second session, participants completed the same measures and were provided a written debriefing statement summarizing the purposes of the research.

Data analysis

We used R (version 4.2.2) to analyze the data [32]. Internal consistency was evaluated using Cronbach’s alpha [35], and McDonald’s Omega [36], using the Coefficient alpha package [37], where > 0.70 is acceptable, > 0.8 is good, and 0.9 > is excellent [38]. Reliability was assessed using Pearson’s correlation between times 1 and 2, where > 0.70 is considered adequate and > 0.80 is considered good. We conducted a paired t-test of the subscales over the three-week interval to further assess stability.

Results

The demographic makeup of the test-retest participants was predominantly white (69.23%) and female (86.0%), with a mean age of 21.05 (SD = 1.35). Participants’ scores on the subscales displayed adequate (NT subscale) to good internal consistency. The Pearson correlations of the participants’ scores were statistically significant. Subscales ND (r = 0.67) and NW (r = 0.66) exhibited questionable test-retest reliability. Only the NW dimension differed in a statistically meaningful way from time 1 (M = 2.12, SD = 0.78) to 2 (M = 2.30, SD = 0.76); t (65) = 2.25, p = 0.03. All other subscales demonstrate adequate temporal stability. See Table 3 for all reported statistics.

Table 3 Time 1 and Time 2: internal consistency, test-retest Pearson Correlations, and T-tests

Discussion

The results of study 2 indicated that respondents’ scores on the dimensions of the MAIA-2 had acceptable internal consistency characteristics and variable temporal stability. Specifically, the NW subscale data suggested that respondents consistently interpret and respond to items for this scale but report variation in their location (i.e., mean values). Moreover, the NW mean value showed a tendency to experience emotional distress by physical discomfort.

General discussion

Our sample demonstrated (1) interpretability of the items, (2) good model fit with modifications and strict invariance between men and women, and (3) generally acceptable temporal stability. The two studies are informative because screening and assessment for attention, concentration, and related mind-body processing phenomena frequently occur during peoples’ college years.

The CFA results show a good degree of fit and retention of all items on their original factors in this college sample, with some minor modifications in the items’ covariances and similar properties to Mehlings et al.’s results [12, 13]. Previous validation studies with college samples demonstrate different factor structures and item retention, such as six factors with Japanese [25, 26] and Lithuanian [22] samples, while a Portuguese sample [24] resulted in seven dimensions.

Our sample appeared to share seemingly similar sample sociodemographics of the MAIA [12], and MAIA-2 [13] (i.e., predominately English-speaking white female participants) and confirmed the factor structure of the measure, providing additional consideration that cultural and contextual factors may impact the subjective experience of interoceptive awareness. Cultural differences have been theorized as influential factors in the subjective nature of interpreting bodily signals [25, 26], thus potentially impacting the psychometrics of the MAIA-2.

Regarding reliability, the findings support consistent interpretation and responses to all dimensions of the MAIA-2. The results implied adequate temporal stability except for the NW and ND subscales, displaying similar results as a Dutch non-clinical sample [44]. The college respondents reported fluctuating tendencies to use distraction to cope with emotional distress from physical discomfort. The NW dimension has displayed negative associations with pain catastrophizing [11, 12], state and trait anxiety [11, 12, 45], somatosensory amplification, and neuroticism [46]. The ND dimension was negatively correlated with difficulty identifying feelings [46], anxiety [11, 12, 17], and positively associated with mindfulness [11, 12]. This might imply that the NW and ND scales are more analogous to state characteristics that contextual factors (e.g., time of the semester) influence and would help explain the variability in its performance across time. Students’ tendency to worry and utilize distraction to regulate is consistent with increased rates of anxiety and other mental health concerns on college campuses [1, 2]. This sample also strongly endorsed a tendency to notice, attend to, assign emotions from, and trust their bodily signals. These findings warrant further investigations into the utilization of somatic therapies, such as somatic reappraisal and mindfulness via interoceptive attention [47].

Limitations

First, we did not evaluate the convergent or discriminate validity. Second, the data was limited to samples from a single US college, raising questions of external validity. Third, the study did not measure participants’ experiences with mind-body practices or mental health conditions; therefore, we could not assess if these practices or conditions influenced the college students’ scores. Beyond these limitations, the MAIA-2 appears to be a generally good measure of interoceptive awareness and one to which people can consistently respond over time.

Data availability

Contact the corresponding author with questions or requests regarding the data in this project.

Abbreviations

NT:

Noticing

ND:

Not-distracting

NW:

Not-worrying

AR:

Attention regulation

EA:

Emotional awareness

SR:

Self-regulation

BL:

Body listening

TR:

Trusting

References

  1. Limone P, Toto GA. Factors that predispose undergraduates to mental issues: A cumulative literature review for future research perspectives. Front Public Health. 2022;10:831349. doi:https://doi.org/10.3389/fpubh.2022.831349.

  2. American Psychological Association (APA). Campus mental health, American Psychological Association., https://www.apa.org/monitor/2022/10/mental-health-campus-care. Accessed June 28, 2022.

  3. Wilkinson CB, Infantolino ZP, Wacha-Monyes A. Evidence-based practice as a potential solution to burnout in university counseling center clinicians. Psychol Serv. 2017;14(4):543–8. https://doi.org/10.1037/ser0000156.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Bamber MD, Schneider JK. College students’ perceptions of mindfulness-based interventions: a narrative review of the qualitative research. Curr Psychol. 2022;4:667–80. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-00592-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Falsafi N. A randomized controlled trial of mindfulness versus yoga: effects on depression and/or anxiety in college students. J am Psychiatr Nurses Assoc. 2016;22(6):483–97. https://doi.org/10.1177/1078390316663397.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. McIndoo CC, File AA, Preddy T, Clark CG, Hopko DR. Mindfulness-based therapy and behavioral activation: a randomized controlled trial with depressed college students. Behav Res Ther. 2016;77:118–29. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2015.12.012.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Mehling WE, Gopisetty V, Daubenmier J, Price CJ, Hecht FM, Stewart A. Body awareness: construct and self-report measures. PLoS ONE. 2009;4(5):e56141–18. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0005614.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. Bornemann B, Herbert BM, Mehling WE, Singer T. Differential changes in self-reported aspects of interoceptive awareness through 3 months of contemplative training. Front Psychol. 2015;5:1–13. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01504.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Khalsa SS, Adolphs R, Cameron OG, Critchley HD, Davenport PW, Feinstein JS et al. Interoception and mental health: A roadmap. Biol Psychiatry. 2018;3(5):501–513. doi.10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.12.004.

  10. Garfinkel SN, Seth AK, Barrett AB, Suzuki K, Critchley HD. Knowing your own heart: distinguishing interoceptive accuracy from interoceptive awareness. Bio Psychol. 2015;104(2015):65–74. https://doi.org/10.1016/biopsycho.2014.11.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Ferentzi E, Olaru G, Geiger M, Vig L, Köteles F, Wihelm O. Examining the factor structure and validity of the multidimensional assessment of interoceptive awareness. J Pers Assess. 2021;103(5):675–84. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2020.1813147.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Mehling WE, Price C, Daubenmier JJ, Acree M, Bartmess E, Stewart A. The multidimensional assessment of interoceptive awareness (MAIA). PLoS ONE. 2012;4(5):e5614. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0005614.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  13. Mehling WE, Acree M, Stewart A, Silas J, Jones A. The Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness, version 2 (Maia-2). PLoS ONE. 2018;13(12). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0208034.

  14. Bullock AJ, Goldbacher EM. Interoceptive awareness and emotional eating in college women: the role of appetite and emotional awareness. J AM Coll Health. 2021. https://doi.org/10.1080/07448481.2021.1970566.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Lovan P, Prado G, Lee T, Coccia CA. Snapshot of eating behaviors in undergraduate college students living in South Florida. J AM Coll Health. 2022. https://doi.org/10.1080/07448481.2022.2119402.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Oswald A, Chapman J, Wilson C. Do interoceptive awareness and interoceptive responsiveness mediate the relationship between body appreciation and intuitive eating in young women? Appetite. 2017;109:66–72. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2016.11.019.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Kabir RS. Interoceptive attention tendencies predict trait anxiety in undergraduate students and hospital nurses participating in stress management programs. Bull Grad School Educ Hiroshima Univ Part III. 2019;68:111–20.

    Google Scholar 

  18. Vig L, Köteles F, Ferentzi E. Questionnaires of interocpetion do not assess the same construct. PLoS ONE. 2022;17(8):e0273299. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.prone.023299.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Reis D. Further insights into the German version of the multidimensional assessment of interoceptive awareness (maia): exploratory and bayesian structural equation modeling approaches. Eur J Psychol Assess. 2019;35(3):317–25. https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000404

  20. Gim W, Sim K, Cho O. Korean multidimensional assessment of interoceptive awareness (K-MAIA): development and validation. Korean J Stress Res. 2016;24(3):177–92. https://doi.org/10.17547/kjsr.2016.3.177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Lin FL, Hsu CC, Mehling W, Yeh ML. Translation and psychometric testing of the Chinese version of the multidimensional assessment of the interoceptive awareness. J Nurs Res 2017;\ 25(1):76–83. doi: 10/1097/jnr.0000000000000182.

  22. Baranauskas M, Grabausaitė A, Griškova-Bulanova. Psychometric characteristics of Lithuanian Version of Multidimensional assessment of interoceptive awareness (maiaLT). Neurologijos Seminarai. 2016;20(70):202–6.

    Google Scholar 

  23. Cali G, Ambronsini E, Picconi L, Mheling WE, Committeri G. Investigating the relationship between interoceptive accuracy, interoceptive awareness, and emotional susceptibility. Front. Psychol. 2015;6(1202). doi.org/fpsyh.2015.01202.

  24. Machorrinho J, Veiga G, Fernandes J, Mehling W, Marmeleria J. Multidimensional assessment of interoceptive awareness: psychometric properties of the Portuguese version. Percept Mot Skills. 2018;126(1):87–105. doi. 10/1177/0031512518813231.

  25. Fujino H. Further validation of the Japanese version of the multidimensional assessment of interoceptive awareness. BMC Res Notes. 2019;12(530). doi.org/10/1186/s13104-019-4556-x

  26. Shoji M, Mehling WE, Hautzinger M, Herbert B. Investigating multidimensional interoceptive awareness in a Japanese population: Validation of the Japanese MAIA-J. Front Psychol 2018;9:1855. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01855

  27. Valenzulela-Moguillansky C, Reyes-Reyes A. Psychometric properties of the multidimensional assessment of interoceptive awareness (maia) in a Chilean population. Front Psychol. 2015;2015(6):120. https://doi.org/10.3389/psyg.2015.00120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Montoya-Hurtado O, Gómez-Jaramillo N, Bermúdez-Jaimes G, et al. Psychometric properties of the Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive awareness (MAIA) questionnaire in Colombian University students. J Clin Med. 2023;12(8):2937. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm12082937. Published 2023 Apr 18.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Todd J, Barron D, Aspell JE, Toh EKL, Zahari HS, Khatib NAM, Swami V. Translation and validation of a Bahasa Malaysia (malay) version of the multidimensional assessment of interoceptive awareness (MAIA). PLoS ONE. 2020;15(4):e0231048. 10.1371journal.pone.0231048.

  30. Osher Center for Integrative Health. Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness. Accessed June 13. 2024. https://osher.ucsf.edu/research/maia

  31. Open Science Collaboration. Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science. Science. 2015;349(aao4716). doi.org/aac4716.

  32. R Core Team. R: a Language and environment for statistical processing [computer software]. Version 4.2.2. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2013.

    Google Scholar 

  33. Little RJA. A test of missing completely at random for multivariate data with missing values. J Am Stat Assoc. 1988;83(404):1198–202. https://doi.org/10.1080/01621459.1988.1047822.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Van Buuren S, Groothuis-Oudshoorn K. Multivariate imputation by chained equations in R. J Stat Softw. 2023;45(3):1–67. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v045.i03.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Cronbach LJ. Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests. Psychometrika. 1951;16(3):297–334. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02310555.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Commun Methods Meas. 2020;14(1):1–24. doi:10.1080/19312458.2020.1718629.

  37. Zhang Z, Yuan KH. Robust coefficient alpha and omega and confidence intervals with outlying observations and missing data: methods and software. Educ Psychol Meas. 2016;76(3):387–411. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164415594658.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Taber KS. The use of Cronbach’s alpha when developing and reporting research instruments in science education. Res Sci Educ. 2018;48:1273–96. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11165-016-9602-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Hu L, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternative. Struct Equ Model. 1999;6(1):1–55. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Kline RB. Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. 3rd ed. New York, NY: Guildford; 2011.

    Google Scholar 

  41. Rosseel Y, Lavaan. An R package for structural equation modeling. J Stat Softw. 2012;48(2):1–36. http://www.jstatsoft.org/.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Putnick DL, Bornstein MH. Measurement invariance conventions and reporting: the state of the art and future directions for psychological research. Dev Rev. 2016;41:71–90. https://doi.org/10/1016/j.dr.2016.06.004

  43. Sass DA. Testing measurement invariance and comparing latent factor means within confirmatory factor analysis framework. J Psychoeduc Assess. 2011;29(4):347–63. https://doi.org/10.1177/0734282911406661.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Scheffers M, Coenen J, Moeijes J, Bellemans T. The multidimensional assessment of interoceptive awareness, version 2 (MAIA-2): psychometric properties in a Dutch non-clinical sample. BMC Psychol. 2024;12(53):1–8. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-024-01553-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Slotta T, Witthöft M, Gerlach AL, Pohl A. The interplay of interoceptive accuracy, facets of interoceptive sensibility, and trait anxiety: a network analysis. Pers Individ Dif. 2018;182:111133. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2021.111133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Gaggero G, Dellantonio S, Pastore L, Sng KHL, Esposito G. Shared and unique interoceptive deficits in high alexithymia and neurocticism. PLoS ONE. 2022;17(8):e0273922. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.prone.0273922.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  47. Price CJ, Weng HY. Facilitating adaptive emotion processing and somatic reappraisal via sustained mindful interoceptive attention. Front Psychol. 2021;12:578827. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.58827.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

This study was not supported by a research grant or other funding.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

HEC designed the study, collected, analyzed, and interpreted the data, and wrote the manuscript. AES designed the study, collected, analyzed, and interpreted the data, and wrote the manuscript. HEC and AES approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Harrison E. Chapman.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

This study was reviewed and approved by the University of Georgia’s Office of Research and Institutional Review Board (STUDY00006902; PROJECT00005184). All methods were carried out in accordance with the University of Georgia’s Office of Research and Institutional Review Board’s relevant guidelines and regulations. All participants were explained the purpose of the study, assured of the protection of their privacy, and provided informed consent. All participants’ informed consent was obtained prior to the administration of the studies.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary Material 1

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Chapman, H.E., Stewart, A.E. Interoceptive awareness in a Southeastern US college sample: validation of the multidimensional assessment of interoceptive awareness – version 2. BMC Res Notes 17, 236 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13104-024-06894-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13104-024-06894-6

Keywords