- Research Note
- Open access
- Published:
Time-course of balance training-related changes on static and dynamic balance performance in healthy children
BMC Research Notes volume 17, Article number: 81 (2024)
Abstract
Objective
In healthy children, there is evidence of improvements in static and dynamic balance performance following balance training. However, the time-course of balance training-related changes is unknown. Thus, we determined the effects of balance training after one, three, and six weeks of exercise on measures of static and dynamic balance in healthy children (N = 44, 20 females, mean age: 9.6 ± 0.5 years, age range: 9–11 years).
Results
Participants in the intervention group (2 × 25 min balance exercises per week) compared to those in the control group (2 × 25 min track and field exercises and soccer practice per week) significantly improved their static (i.e., by measuring stance time in the One-Legged Stance test) and dynamic (i.e., by counting step number in the 3-m Beam Walking Backward test) balance performance. Late effects (after 6 weeks) occurred most frequently followed by mid-term effects (after 3 weeks) and then early effects (after 1 week). These findings imply that balance training is effective to improve static and dynamic measures of balance in healthy children, whereby the effectiveness increases with increasing training period.
Trial registration
Current Controlled Trials ISRCTN16518737 (retrospectively registered at 24th August, 2023).
Introduction
There is evidence from original studies [1, 2] and a systematic review with meta-analysis [3] that balance training (BT) is effective to improve static and dynamic balance in healthy children. For instance, Walchli et al. [1] detected significantly decreased postural sway during the One-Legged Stance (OLS) test on a spinning top after five weeks of BT (2 times/week) in children aged 6–7 years and 11–12 years. Moreover, Schedler et al. [2] reported significantly increased reach distances for the Y Balance Test-Lower Quarter in 10–11 year-old children following eight weeks of BT (2 times/week). However, these studies used a pre-post-design separated by several weeks and do therefore not provide any insights into the time-course of balance training-related changes in youth. For an optimal planning of training schedules with limited time to promote postural control at school (during P.E.) or in sports clubs, however, it is important to know whether BT effects occur early on or only in the long-term.
In fact, the time-course of changes in balance with training has so far only been studied in older adults [4,5,6]. For instance, Alizadehsaravi et al. [4] compared balance robustness (duration of balancing) and balance performance (centre of mass velocity) after one and ten training sessions in adults aged ≥ 65 years. The authors already observed significant improvements in both measures after one session as well as after ten sessions. Yet, transferring these findings to children is not legitimate, as older adults are subject to age-related degeneration processes in the sensory and motor systems [7,8,9]. In addition, due to growth, maturation, and development, children do not yet have a fully developed postural control system [10, 11].
Therefore, the aim of the study was to investigate the time-course of balance training-related changes (i.e., after 1, 3, and 6 weeks of BT) on measures of static and dynamic balance in healthy children. We hypothesised that BT would result in balance improvements, which take place in as little as two sessions per week (i.e., after 1 week of BT).
Methods
Participants
An a priori power analysis (f = 0.25, α = 0.05, 1-β = 0.80, r =.25, 2 groups, 2 assessments, drop-out rate of 20% due to reasons not attributable to treatment) revealed that a total sample size of N = 42 participants (i.e., n = 21 per group) would be sufficient to detect significant within-between interactions [12]. Therefore, 44 children were recruited and randomly assigned to the intervention (INT) group or control (CON) group (Table 1). All individuals participated voluntarily in the study and had no neurological or musculoskeletal impairment. None of the participants had prior experience with the applied balance tests. Written informed consent and subject’s assent were obtained from all participants before the start of the study. In addition, parent’s approval was obtained for minors.
Balance assessment
The same skilled assessors (graduated sport scientists) conducted the balance assessments before training (pre) and after one (post 1 = early effects), three (post 2 = mid-term effects), and six (post 3 = late effects) weeks of training. The timed OLS test was used to assess static balance. Participants were asked to stand on their non-dominant leg (determined by self-report) for as long as possible but for a maximum of 60 s with (a) eyes closed on firm ground (EC-FI = supressed vision/proprioception dominant), (b) eyes opened on foam (i.e., AIREX balance pad) ground (EO-FO = vision dominant/modified proprioception), and (c) eyes closed on foam ground (EC-FO = supressed vision and modified proprioception/vestibular dominant). Following a practice trial, one data-collection trial was executed, and the maximal stance time (s) during each condition was used for further analysis. The timed OLS test is valid (concurrent and discriminative) and reliable (moderate to excellent) in youth [13, 14].
The 3-m Beam Walking Backward test [15] was used to determine dynamic balance. Participants were asked to walk backward at a self-selected speed from the beginning to the end of wooden beams (length: 3 m; height: 5 cm) that differed in width (i.e., 6.0, 4.5, and 3.0 cm) for a minimum of eight steps. After one practice trial, two data-collection trials per beam width were performed. The number of steps for the data-collection trials was added up resulting in a maximum of 16 steps per beam width. The 3-m Beam Walk test is valid (content, construct, and criterion-related) and reliable (moderate) in youth [15].
Interventions
Both groups trained for six weeks (i.e., 2 × 25 min per week on non-consecutive days) in a group scenario that was supervised by two graduated students. The participants of the INT-group performed static (e.g., bipedal/tandem/unipedal stance), dynamic (e.g., balancing for-/back-/sideward), proactive (e.g., weight shifting for-/back-/sideward and reaching for-/back-/sideward with one leg/arm), and reactive (e.g., push/pull while standing/walking) balance exercises during physical education. Two to three trials per exercise (each trial lasted 30–45 s) were conducted. The rest period between trials and exercises was 30 and 45 s, respectively. Training progression was achieved by a gradual increase of trial number and duration. The participants in the CON-group conducted three weeks of soccer practice followed by three weeks of track and field exercises.
Statistical analyses
Data were analysed using Statistical Package for Social Sciences (version 27.0) and reported as group means ± standard deviations. After normal distribution was confirmed (i.e., Shapiro-Wilk tests), a 2 (group: INT-group, CON-group) × 4 (test: pre, post 1, post 2, post 3) repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted to detect significant within-between interactions. GLM contrasts (type: simple) were analysed to investigate the time-course of changes from pre (means the reference category) to post 1, post 2, and post 3. Further, effect size (ηp2) was calculated and reported as small (0.02 ≤ ηp2 ≤ 0.12), medium (0.13 ≤ ηp2 ≤ 0.25), or large (ηp2 ≥ 0.26) [16]. The α-value was a priori set at p <.05.
Results
Static balance performance
For all but one (i.e., EO–FO = vision dominant/ modified proprioception) stance condition, the repeated measures ANOVA (Table 2) showed significant main effects of ‘Test’ (i.e., differences between the pretest and posttests, irrespective of group). ‘Group × test’ interactions (i.e., group-specific differences between the pretest and posttests) were detected for the EC–FI (supressed vision/proprioception dominant) and the EO–FO (vision dominant/ modified proprioception) stance conditions. GLM contrasts revealed significant mid-term and late improvements for the INT-group from pre to post 2 (EC–FI: p =.021, ηp2 = 0.23) and post 3 (EC–FI: p <.001, ηp2 = 0.55; EO–FO: p =.014, ηp2 = 0.26) but not for the CON group (Fig. 1A–C). The main effect of ‘Group’ (i.e., differences between the INT-group and CON-group, irrespective of test) did not reach the level of significance, irrespective of stance condition.
Dynamic balance performance
A significant main effect of ‘Test’ was only detected for the 6.0-cm beam width (Table 2). ‘Group × test’ interactions were observed for the 4.5-cm and 3-cm beam width. GLM contrasts revealed significant early, mid-term, and late improvements for the INT-group from pre to post 1 (3.0-cm: p =.013, ηp2 = 0.26), post 2 (3.0-cm: p =.006, ηp2 = 0.30), and post 3 (4.5-cm: p <.001, ηp2 = 0.47; 3.0-cm: p <.001, ηp2 = 0.52) but not for the CON group (Fig. 1D–F). A significant main effect of ‘Group’ was only found for the 3.0-cm beam width.
Please insert Fig. 1A–F about here.
Discussion
In agreement with the first part of our hypothesis, we detected significant improvements in static (increased stance time in the OLS test) and dynamic (increased number of steps in the 3-m Beam Walking Backward test) balance for the INT-group compared to the CON-group. These findings are consistent with those from previous studies [1, 2] that investigated the effect of BT in healthy children and indicate that BT is an effective training regimen to enhance static and dynamic balance in this age group.
Late effects after six weeks of BT (i.e., 12 sessions) occurred most frequently followed by mid-term effects (after 3 weeks of BT = 6 sessions) and then early effects (after 1 week of BT = 2 sessions); confirming the second part of our hypothesis. This finding implies that BT is an effective approach to improve measures of static and dynamic measures of balance performance, whereby the effectiveness increases with increasing training period. Only for the most difficult stance condition (EC–FO) and the easiest walking condition (6.0-cm beam width), we did not detect significant improvements, indicating a ‘floor’ and ‘ceiling’ effect, respectively. Apparently, in the first case, the combination of standing with eyes closed on foam ground was too difficult, which limited the potential for enhancements. In the second case, the number of steps was already close to the maximum of 16, leaving little room for further improvements.
To our knowledge, this study is the first one that investigated the time-course of balance training-related changes in children. Consequently, the results can only be interpreted in the context of studies that examined different age groups. In previous publications on older adults, static and dynamic balance performance already improved after a single session of training [4, 5] but improved gradually over multiple sessions [4, 6]. In summary, these and our findings suggest that BT leads to improvements early on, but that the potential for adaptations can be further utilized with several training sessions. From a practitioner’s perspective, BT is an effective regimen for improving static and dynamic balance performance after only a few sessions and can therefore already be used with limited time resources. However, at least in children longer training periods seem to be beneficial to produce greater adaptations.
With regard to the underlying mechanisms, it can be speculated, based on studies with adults, that these take place at the spinal and supraspinal level [17]. Specifically, decreasing reflex activities [18, 19] as well as structural and functional brain changes [20, 21] were shown as a result of long- but also short-term periods of BT. Future studies should examine whether these mechanisms also occur in children and adolescents.
Conclusion
This study investigated the early, mid, and late effects of BT in healthy children. We found significant improvements in static (i.e., increased stance duration in the OLS test) and dynamic (i.e., increased number of steps in the 3-m Beam Walking Backward) balance for the intervention compared to the active control group. Most frequently, late effects (after 6 weeks of training) occurred followed by mid-term (after 3 weeks of training) and then early effects (after 1 week of training). These results imply that BT is an effective training regimen in healthy children and the effectiveness of BT increases with increasing training period.
Limitations
-
Only children were investigated, which limits the generalisation of findings to adolescents.
-
No laboratory-based testing (e.g., postural sway via force-plate) was applied, which limits the internal validity.
-
The time-course of balance training-related changes was determined on a behavioural but not on a neuronal level (i.e., brain/muscle activation).
Data availability
The data generated and analysed during the present study are not publicly available due to ethical restrictions but are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.
Abbreviations
- ANOVA:
-
Analysis of variance
- BT:
-
Balance training
- CON:
-
Control group
- EC:
-
Eyes closed
- EO:
-
Eyes opened
- FI:
-
Firm ground
- FO:
-
Foam ground
- INT:
-
Intervention group
- OLS:
-
One-legged stance test
- PHV:
-
peak height velocity
References
Walchli M, Ruffieux J, Mouthon A, Keller M, Taube W. Is young age a limiting factor when training balance? Effects of child-oriented balance training in children and adolescents. Pediatr Exerc Sci. 2018;30(1):176–84.
Schedler S, Graf SM, Muehlbauer T. Effects of different balance training volumes on children’s dynamic balance. Sports Med Int Open. 2022;6(1):E32–8.
Gebel A, Lesinski M, Behm DG, Granacher U. Effects and dose-response relationship of balance training on balance performance in youth: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sports Med. 2018;48(9):2067–89.
Alizadehsaravi L, Koster RAJ, Muijres W, Maas H, Bruijn SM, van Dieen JH. The underlying mechanisms of improved balance after one and ten sessions of balance training in older adults. Hum Mov Sci. 2022;81:102910.
Rieger MM, Papegaaij S, Pijnappels M, Steenbrink F, van Dieen JH. Transfer and retention effects of gait training with anterior-posterior perturbations to postural responses after medio-lateral gait perturbations in older adults. Clin Biomech (Bristol Avon). 2020;75:104988.
Alizadehsaravi L, Bruijn SM, Muijres W, Koster RAJ, van Dieen JH. Improvement in gait stability in older adults after ten sessions of standing balance training. PLoS ONE. 2022;17(7):e0242115.
Kararizou E, Manta P, Kalfakis N, Vassilopoulos D. Morphometric study of the human muscle spindle. Anal Quant Cytol Histol. 2005;27(1):1–4.
Morisawa Y. Morphological study of mechanoreceptors on the coracoacromial ligament. J Orthop Sci. 1998;3(2):102–10.
McNeil CJ, Doherty TJ, Stashuk DW, Rice CL. Motor unit number estimates in the tibialis anterior muscle of young, old, and very old men. Muscle Nerve. 2005;31(4):461–7.
Riach CL, Hayes KC. Maturation of postural sway in young children. Dev Med Child Neurol. 1987;29(5):650–8.
Woollacott MH, Shumway-Cook A. Maturation of feedback control of posture and equilibrium. In: Motor development in children. Edited by Fedrizzi E, Avanzini G, Crenna P. New Barnet: John Libbey & Company Ltd; 1994: 59–70.
Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang AG, Buchner A. G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods. 2007;39(2):175–91.
Barozzi S, Socci M, Soi D, Di Berardino F, Fabio G, Forti S, Gasbarre AM, Brambilla D, Cesarani A. Reliability of postural control measures in children and young adolescents. Eur Arch Otorhinolaryngol. 2014;271(7):2069–77.
Zumbrunn T, MacWilliams BA, Johnson BA. Evaluation of a single leg stance balance test in children. Gait Posture. 2011;34(2):174–7.
Bös K. Deutscher Motorik-Test 6–18. Hamburg: Czwalina; 2009.
Cohen J. Statistical power for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1988.
Taube W, Gruber M, Gollhofer A. Spinal and supraspinal adaptations associated with balance training and their functional relevance. Acta Physiol (Oxf). 2008;193(2):101–16.
Gruber M, Taube W, Gollhofer A, Beck S, Amtage F, Schubert M. Training-specific adaptations of H- and stretch reflexes in human soleus muscle. J Mot Behav. 2007;39(1):68–78.
Mynark RG, Koceja DM. Down training of the elderly soleus H reflex with the use of a spinally induced balance perturbation. J Appl Physiol. 2002;93(1):127–33.
Surgent OJ, Dadalko OI, Pickett KA, Travers BG. Balance and the brain: a review of structural brain correlates of postural balance and balance training in humans. Gait Posture. 2019;71:245–52.
Taubert M, Mehnert J, Pleger B, Villringer A. Rapid and specific gray matter changes in M1 induced by balance training. NeuroImage. 2016;133:399–407.
Acknowledgements
Not applicable.
Funding
The support by the Open Access Publication Fund of the University of Duisburg-Essen is acknowledged. The funding body is independent of the design of the study and collection, analysis, and interpretation of data and in writing the manuscript. Open access funding enabled and organized by the project DEAL.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
All authors designed the research question. MG and NR conducted the testing and data collections. MG and NR planned and supervised the interventions. TM, SS, and MWH analysed the data. TM, SS and MWH wrote the main parts of the manuscript. All the authors contributed to critical review of draft manuscripts. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Ethics approval and consent to participate
Ethical permission was given by the Human Ethics Committee at the University of Duisburg-Essen, Faculty of Educational Sciences. Participants’ assent and parents’ written informed consent were obtained prior to the start of the study. All authors confirm that all methods were performed in accordance with the relevant guidelines and regulations.
Consent for publication
Not applicable.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Author details
1Division of Movement and Training Sciences/Biomechanics of Sport, University of Duisburg-Essen, Essen, Germany.
2Centre for Physical Activity, Sport and Exercise Sciences, Coventry University, Coventry, United Kingdom.
Additional information
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.
About this article
Cite this article
Muehlbauer, T., Giesen, M., Roß, N. et al. Time-course of balance training-related changes on static and dynamic balance performance in healthy children. BMC Res Notes 17, 81 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13104-024-06745-4
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13104-024-06745-4